BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

173 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10509881)

  • 41. Absence of spontaneous action anticipation by false belief attribution in children with autism spectrum disorder.
    Senju A; Southgate V; Miura Y; Matsui T; Hasegawa T; Tojo Y; Osanai H; Csibra G
    Dev Psychopathol; 2010 May; 22(2):353-60. PubMed ID: 20423546
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Executive function and the development of belief-desire psychology.
    Rakoczy H
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jul; 13(4):648-61. PubMed ID: 20590728
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. What's domain-specific about theory of mind?
    Stone VE; Gerrans P
    Soc Neurosci; 2006; 1(3-4):309-19. PubMed ID: 18633796
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. A continuous false belief task reveals egocentric biases in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders.
    Begeer S; Bernstein DM; van Wijhe J; Scheeren AM; Koot HM
    Autism; 2012 Jul; 16(4):357-66. PubMed ID: 22399450
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Theory of mind and executive function: working-memory capacity and inhibitory control as predictors of false-belief task performance.
    Mutter B; Alcorn MB; Welsh M
    Percept Mot Skills; 2006 Jun; 102(3):819-35. PubMed ID: 16916162
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Re-examining the cognitive phenotype in autism: a study with young Chinese children.
    Lam YG
    Res Dev Disabil; 2013 Dec; 34(12):4591-8. PubMed ID: 24171826
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Reading minds versus following rules: dissociating theory of mind and executive control in the brain.
    Saxe R; Schulz LE; Jiang YV
    Soc Neurosci; 2006; 1(3-4):284-98. PubMed ID: 18633794
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Tracking speakers' false beliefs: is theory of mind available earlier for word learning?
    Houston-Price C; Goddard K; Séclier C; Grant SC; Reid CJ; Boyden LE; Williams R
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jul; 14(4):623-34. PubMed ID: 21676084
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Preschoolers' implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: relations with complex syntactical mastery.
    Low J
    Child Dev; 2010; 81(2):597-615. PubMed ID: 20438463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Social cognition is not reducible to theory of mind: when children use deontic rules to predict the behaviour of others.
    Clément F; Bernard S; Kaufmann L
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Nov; 29(Pt 4):910-28. PubMed ID: 21995744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Using tests of false belief with children with autism: how valid and reliable are they?
    Grant CM; Grayson A; Boucher J
    Autism; 2001 Jun; 5(2):135-45. PubMed ID: 11706862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Theory of mind and self-control: more than a common problem of inhibition.
    Perner J; Lang B; Kloo D
    Child Dev; 2002; 73(3):752-67. PubMed ID: 12038549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Children with autism can track others' beliefs in a competitive game.
    Peterson CC; Slaughter V; Peterson J; Premack D
    Dev Sci; 2013 May; 16(3):443-50. PubMed ID: 23587041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Language and theory of mind in autism spectrum disorder: the relationship between complement syntax and false belief task performance.
    Lind SE; Bowler DM
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2009 Jun; 39(6):929-37. PubMed ID: 19205856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Counterfactual and mental state reasoning in children with autism.
    Grant CM; Riggs KJ; Boucher J
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2004 Apr; 34(2):177-88. PubMed ID: 15162936
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Thought-bubbles help children with autism acquire an alternative to a theory of mind.
    Wellman HM; Baron-Cohen S; Caswell R; Gomez JC; Swettenham J; Toye E; Lagattuta K
    Autism; 2002 Dec; 6(4):343-63. PubMed ID: 12540127
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism.
    Hughes C; Russell J; Robbins TW
    Neuropsychologia; 1994 Apr; 32(4):477-92. PubMed ID: 8047253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Executive function in parents of children with autism.
    Hughes C; Leboyer M; Bouvard M
    Psychol Med; 1997 Jan; 27(1):209-20. PubMed ID: 9122301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Photographic cues do not always facilitate performance on false belief tasks in children with autism.
    Bowler DM; Briskman JA
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2000 Aug; 30(4):305-16. PubMed ID: 11039857
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Drawing insight from pictures: the development of concepts of false drawing and false belief in children with deafness, normal hearing, and autism.
    Peterson CC
    Child Dev; 2002; 73(5):1442-59. PubMed ID: 12361311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.