These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

166 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10553673)

  • 1. Newborn infants' sensitivity to perceptual cues to lexical and grammatical words.
    Shi R; Werker JF; Morgan JL
    Cognition; 1999 Sep; 72(2):B11-21. PubMed ID: 10553673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Six-month-old infants' preference for lexical words.
    Shi R; Werker JF
    Psychol Sci; 2001 Jan; 12(1):70-5. PubMed ID: 11294231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. [Speech perception in the first two years].
    Bertoncini J; Cabrera L
    Arch Pediatr; 2014 Oct; 21(10):1153-6. PubMed ID: 25218761
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Auditory Discrimination of Lexical Stress Patterns in Hearing-Impaired Infants with Cochlear Implants Compared with Normal Hearing: Influence of Acoustic Cues and Listening Experience to the Ambient Language.
    Segal O; Houston D; Kishon-Rabin L
    Ear Hear; 2016; 37(2):225-34. PubMed ID: 26627470
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Audiovisual speech perception in infancy: The influence of vowel identity and infants' productive abilities on sensitivity to (mis)matches between auditory and visual speech cues.
    Altvater-Mackensen N; Mani N; Grossmann T
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Feb; 52(2):191-204. PubMed ID: 26595352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Narrowing the distance to language: one step at a time.
    Jusczyk PW
    J Commun Disord; 1999; 32(4):207-22. PubMed ID: 10466094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The differential role of phonological and distributional cues in grammatical categorisation.
    Monaghan P; Chater N; Christiansen MH
    Cognition; 2005 Jun; 96(2):143-82. PubMed ID: 15925574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Infants' sensitivity to non-adjacent dependencies across phonological phrase boundaries.
    van Heugten M; Shi R
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2010 Nov; 128(5):EL223-8. PubMed ID: 21110530
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Exploring the "anchor word" effect in infants: Segmentation and categorisation of speech with and without high frequency words.
    Frost RLA; Dunn K; Christiansen MH; Gómez RL; Monaghan P
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(12):e0243436. PubMed ID: 33332419
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Do infants perceive word boundaries? An empirical study of the bootstrapping of lexical acquisition.
    Christophe A; Dupoux E; Bertoncini J; Mehler J
    J Acoust Soc Am; 1994 Mar; 95(3):1570-80. PubMed ID: 8176060
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The development of a phonological illusion: a cross-linguistic study with Japanese and French infants.
    Mazuka R; Cao Y; Dupoux E; Christophe A
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jul; 14(4):693-9. PubMed ID: 21676090
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Prosody and Function Words Cue the Acquisition of Word Meanings in 18-Month-Old Infants.
    de Carvalho A; He AX; Lidz J; Christophe A
    Psychol Sci; 2019 Mar; 30(3):319-332. PubMed ID: 30668928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Infants' sensitivity to allophonic cues for word segmentation.
    Jusczyk PW; Hohne EA; Bauman A
    Percept Psychophys; 1999 Nov; 61(8):1465-76. PubMed ID: 10598463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. No perceptual reorganization for Limburgian tones? A cross-linguistic investigation with 6- to 12-month-old infants.
    Ramachers S; Brouwer S; Fikkert P
    J Child Lang; 2018 Mar; 45(2):290-318. PubMed ID: 28615089
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Twenty-two-month-olds discriminate fluent from disfluent adult-directed speech.
    Soderstrom M; Morgan JL
    Dev Sci; 2007 Sep; 10(5):641-53. PubMed ID: 17683348
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Rhythm in language acquisition.
    Langus A; Mehler J; Nespor M
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2017 Oct; 81(Pt B):158-166. PubMed ID: 27993604
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech.
    Mintz TH
    Cognition; 2003 Nov; 90(1):91-117. PubMed ID: 14597271
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. From babble to words: Infants' early productions match words and objects in their environment.
    Laing C; Bergelson E
    Cogn Psychol; 2020 Nov; 122():101308. PubMed ID: 32504852
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The role of prosody in infants' early syntactic analysis and grammatical categorization.
    Massicotte-Laforge S; Shi R
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2015 Oct; 138(4):EL441-6. PubMed ID: 26520358
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.