These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

107 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10637619)

  • 1. The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy.
    Johnson SC
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2000 Jan; 4(1):22-28. PubMed ID: 10637619
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Young children's attribution of action to beliefs and desires.
    Bartsch K; Wellman H
    Child Dev; 1989 Aug; 60(4):946-64. PubMed ID: 2758888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Understanding individual differences in theory of mind via representation of minds, not mental states.
    Conway JR; Catmur C; Bird G
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2019 Jun; 26(3):798-812. PubMed ID: 30652239
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Psychological Reasoning in Infancy.
    Baillargeon R; Scott RM; Bian L
    Annu Rev Psychol; 2016; 67():159-86. PubMed ID: 26393869
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The impact of extensive medial frontal lobe damage on 'Theory of Mind' and cognition.
    Bird CM; Castelli F; Malik O; Frith U; Husain M
    Brain; 2004 Apr; 127(Pt 4):914-28. PubMed ID: 14998913
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Infants understand deceptive intentions to implant false beliefs about identity: New evidence for early mentalistic reasoning.
    Scott RM; Richman JC; Baillargeon R
    Cogn Psychol; 2015 Nov; 82():32-56. PubMed ID: 26374383
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Do children prefer mentalistic descriptions?
    Dore RA; Lillard AS
    J Genet Psychol; 2014; 175(1-2):1-15. PubMed ID: 24796151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Perspectives on Perspective Taking: How Children Think About the Minds of Others.
    Birch SA; Li V; Haddock T; Ghrear SE; Brosseau-Liard P; Baimel A; Whyte M
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2017; 52():185-226. PubMed ID: 28215285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Probing the depth of infants' theory of mind: disunity in performance across paradigms.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Yott J
    Dev Sci; 2018 Jul; 21(4):e12600. PubMed ID: 28952180
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Functional imaging of 'theory of mind'.
    Gallagher HL; Frith CD
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2003 Feb; 7(2):77-83. PubMed ID: 12584026
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.
    Carpenter M; Nagell K; Tomasello M
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1998; 63(4):i-vi, 1-143. PubMed ID: 9835078
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Infants' social and motor experience and the emerging understanding of intentional actions.
    Brandone AC
    Dev Psychol; 2015 Apr; 51(4):512-23. PubMed ID: 25689000
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy.
    Vouloumanos A; Waxman SR
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2014 Dec; 18(12):642-6. PubMed ID: 25457376
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs?
    Onishi KH; Baillargeon R
    Science; 2005 Apr; 308(5719):255-8. PubMed ID: 15821091
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Understanding of Goals, Beliefs, and Desires Predicts Morally Relevant Theory of Mind: A Longitudinal Investigation.
    Sodian B; Licata M; Kristen-Antonow S; Paulus M; Killen M; Woodward A
    Child Dev; 2016 Jul; 87(4):1221-32. PubMed ID: 27091804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task.
    Buttelmann D; Over H; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():120-6. PubMed ID: 24267393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy.
    Csibra G
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2003 Mar; 358(1431):447-58. PubMed ID: 12689372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Neural signatures for sustaining object representations attributed to others in preverbal human infants.
    Kampis D; Parise E; Csibra G; Kovács ÁM
    Proc Biol Sci; 2015 Nov; 282(1819):. PubMed ID: 26559949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Social elements as mind.
    Harré R
    Br J Med Psychol; 1984 Jun; 57 ( Pt 2)():127-35. PubMed ID: 6743593
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Detecting continuity violations in infancy: a new account and new evidence from covering and tube events.
    Wang SH; Baillargeon R; Paterson S
    Cognition; 2005 Mar; 95(2):129-73. PubMed ID: 15694644
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.