These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
132 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10924238)
1. Conscious and unconscious metacognition: A rejoinder. Koriat A; Levy-Sadot R Conscious Cogn; 2000 Jun; 9(2 Pt 1):193-202. PubMed ID: 10924238 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. The feeling of knowing: some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control. Koriat A Conscious Cogn; 2000 Jun; 9(2 Pt 1):149-71. PubMed ID: 10924234 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Feelings and judgments of knowing: is there a special noetic state? Metcalfe J Conscious Cogn; 2000 Jun; 9(2 Pt 1):178-86; discussion 193-202. PubMed ID: 10924236 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. The dissociation paradigm and its discontents: how can unconscious perception or memory be inferred? Snodgrass M Conscious Cogn; 2004 Mar; 13(1):107-16. PubMed ID: 14990245 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Neuropharmacology of cognition and memory: a unifying theory of neuromodulator imbalance in psychiatry and amnesia. Vakalopoulos C Med Hypotheses; 2006; 66(2):394-431. PubMed ID: 16300905 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. The intricate relationships between monitoring and control in metacognition: lessons for the cause-and-effect relation between subjective experience and behavior. Koriat A; Ma'ayan H; Nussinson R J Exp Psychol Gen; 2006 Feb; 135(1):36-69. PubMed ID: 16478315 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Access is mainly a second-order process: SDT models whether phenomenally (first-order) conscious states are accessed by reflectively (second-order) conscious processes. Snodgrass M; Kalaida N; Winer ES Conscious Cogn; 2009 Jun; 18(2):561-4; discussion 565-7. PubMed ID: 19243977 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Disambiguating conscious and unconscious influences: do exclusion paradigms demonstrate unconscious perception? Snodgrass M Am J Psychol; 2002; 115(4):545-79. PubMed ID: 12516528 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Temporal lobe epilepsy as a model to understand human memory: the distinction between explicit and implicit memory. Leritz EC; Grande LJ; Bauer RM Epilepsy Behav; 2006 Aug; 9(1):1-13. PubMed ID: 16759913 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Metacognition and Mindfulness: the Role of Fringe Consciousness. Norman E Mindfulness (N Y); 2017; 8(1):95-100. PubMed ID: 28163796 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Measures of metacognition on signal-detection theoretic models. Barrett AB; Dienes Z; Seth AK Psychol Methods; 2013 Dec; 18(4):535-52. PubMed ID: 24079931 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Toward a cognitive neuroscience of metacognition. Shimamura AP Conscious Cogn; 2000 Jun; 9(2 Pt 1):313-23; discussion 324-6. PubMed ID: 10924251 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. The role of metacognitive experiences in the learning process. Efklides A Psicothema; 2009 Feb; 21(1):76-82. PubMed ID: 19178860 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. A theory of blindsight--the anatomy of the unconscious: a proposal for the koniocellular projections and intralaminar thalamus. Vakalopoulos C Med Hypotheses; 2005; 65(6):1183-90. PubMed ID: 16054302 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Subjective measures of unconscious knowledge. Dienes Z Prog Brain Res; 2008; 168():49-64. PubMed ID: 18166385 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Cognitive, perceptual and action-oriented representations of falling objects. Zago M; Lacquaniti F Neuropsychologia; 2005; 43(2):178-88. PubMed ID: 15707903 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. The memorizing effort heuristic in judgments of learning: a developmental perspective. Koriat A; Ackerman R; Lockl K; Schneider W J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Mar; 102(3):265-79. PubMed ID: 19084238 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]