These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

189 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10953766)

  • 1. Vertical and bisection bias in active touch.
    Millar S; al-Attar Z
    Perception; 2000; 29(4):481-500. PubMed ID: 10953766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Length perception of horizontal and vertical bisected lines.
    Charras P; Lupiáñez J
    Psychol Res; 2010 Mar; 74(2):196-206. PubMed ID: 19452164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The tactual horizontal-vertical illusion depends on radial motion of the entire arm.
    Heller MA; Calcaterra JA; Burson LL; Green SL
    Percept Psychophys; 1997 Nov; 59(8):1297-311. PubMed ID: 9401462
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The horizontal-vertical illusion: independence of line bisection and a comparison line.
    McBride SA; Risser JM; Slotnick BM
    Percept Mot Skills; 1987 Jun; 64(3 Pt 1):943-8. PubMed ID: 3601614
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The Müller-Lyer illusion in touch and vision: implications for multisensory processes.
    Millar S; Al-Attar Z
    Percept Psychophys; 2002 Apr; 64(3):353-65. PubMed ID: 12049277
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Visual and haptic perception of the horizontal-vertical illusion.
    Taylor CM
    Percept Mot Skills; 2001 Feb; 92(1):167-70. PubMed ID: 11322582
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Illusions in reading maps by touch: reducing distance errors.
    Millar S; Al-Attar Z
    Br J Psychol; 2001 Nov; 92(Pt 4):643-57. PubMed ID: 11762866
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. More far is more right: Manual and ocular line bisections, but not the Judd illusion, depend on radial space.
    Rinaldi L; Bertolini G; Bockisch CJ; Maravita A; Girelli L; Brugger P
    Brain Cogn; 2018 Apr; 122():34-44. PubMed ID: 29428805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Exploring the syndrome of spatial unilateral neglect through an illusion of length.
    Daini R; Angelelli P; Antonucci G; Cappa SF; Vallar G
    Exp Brain Res; 2002 May; 144(2):224-37. PubMed ID: 12012160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The vertical-horizontal illusion in hemi-spatial neglect.
    de Montalembert M; Mamassian P
    Neuropsychologia; 2010 Sep; 48(11):3245-51. PubMed ID: 20624409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The horizontal-vertical illusion and knowledge of results.
    Brosvic GM; Cohen BD
    Percept Mot Skills; 1988 Oct; 67(2):463-9. PubMed ID: 3217193
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Retinal and extraretinal information in movement perception: how to invert the Filehne illusion.
    Wertheim AH
    Perception; 1987; 16(3):299-308. PubMed ID: 3432027
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The relationship between vertical stimulation and horizontal attentional asymmetries.
    Nicholls ME; Thomas NA; Loetscher T; Wignall S; Yates MJ; Forte JD; Spence CJ
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(12):2384-96. PubMed ID: 22650990
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Geometrical haptic illusions: the role of exploration in the Müller-Lyer, vertical-horizontal, and Delboeuf illusions.
    Gentaz E; Hatwell Y
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2004 Feb; 11(1):31-40. PubMed ID: 15116983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Dynamic properties of radial and tangential movements as determinants of the haptic horizontal--vertical illusion with an L figure.
    Wong TS
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 1977 Feb; 3(1):151-64. PubMed ID: 845556
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Why we move to the right? The dominant hand motor-spatial bias.
    Daini R; Vallar G; Arduino LS
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2018 Oct; 147(10):1488-1502. PubMed ID: 30272464
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Analysis of individual variations in the classical horizontal-vertical illusion.
    Hamburger K; Hansen T
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 May; 72(4):1045-52. PubMed ID: 20436199
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Visual illusion and line bisection: a bias hypothesis revisited.
    Chieffi S
    Exp Brain Res; 2016 Jun; 234(6):1451-8. PubMed ID: 26781491
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Tactile flow explains haptic counterparts of common visual illusions.
    Bicchi A; Scilingo EP; Ricciardi E; Pietrini P
    Brain Res Bull; 2008 Apr; 75(6):737-41. PubMed ID: 18394519
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. A coordinate system for visual motion perception.
    Darling WG; Pizzimenti MA
    Exp Brain Res; 2001 Nov; 141(2):174-83. PubMed ID: 11713629
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.