These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10976602)

  • 1. Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: understanding people and their actions.
    Legerstee M; Barna J; DiAdamo C
    Dev Psychol; 2000 Sep; 36(5):627-634. PubMed ID: 10976602
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Infants' understanding of object-directed action.
    Phillips AT; Wellman HM
    Cognition; 2005 Dec; 98(2):137-55. PubMed ID: 16307956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach.
    Woodward AL
    Cognition; 1998 Nov; 69(1):1-34. PubMed ID: 9871370
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Can we talk to robots? Ten-month-old infants expected interactive humanoid robots to be talked to by persons.
    Arita A; Hiraki K; Kanda T; Ishiguro H
    Cognition; 2005 Apr; 95(3):B49-57. PubMed ID: 15788157
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Encoding the goal of an object-directed but uncompleted reaching action in 6- and 9-month-old infants.
    Daum MM; Prinz W; Aschersleben G
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jul; 11(4):607-19. PubMed ID: 18576968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Can infants attribute to an agent a disposition to perform a particular action?
    Song HJ; Baillargeon R; Fisher C
    Cognition; 2005 Dec; 98(2):B45-55. PubMed ID: 15993398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Four-month-old infants' sensitivity to binocular and kinetic information for three-dimensional-object shape.
    Yonas A; Arterberry ME; Granrud CE
    Child Dev; 1987 Aug; 58(4):910-7. PubMed ID: 3608662
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The role of good form in young Infants' perception of partly occluded objects.
    Johnson SP; Bremner JG; Slater AM; Mason UC
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2000 May; 76(1):1-25. PubMed ID: 10764523
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Do Great Minds Prefer Alike? Thirteen-Month-Old Infants Generalize Personal Preferences Across Objects of Like Kind but Not Across People.
    Liu S; Sun R
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():2636. PubMed ID: 30619021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infants recognize similar goals across dissimilar actions involving object manipulation.
    Olofson EL; Baldwin D
    Cognition; 2011 Feb; 118(2):258-64. PubMed ID: 21134672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Behavioral Intentions when Inferring Others' Social Preferences.
    Lee YE; Yun JE; Kim EY; Song HJ
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0135588. PubMed ID: 26383160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded.
    Aguiar A; Baillargeon R
    Cogn Psychol; 1999 Sep; 39(2):116-57. PubMed ID: 10462457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. How infants relate looker and object: evidence for a perceptual learning account of gaze following in infancy.
    Paulus M
    Dev Sci; 2011 Nov; 14(6):1301-10. PubMed ID: 22010890
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Six-month-olds' ability to use linguistic cues when interpreting others' pointing actions.
    Cho I; Lee Y; Song HJ
    Infant Behav Dev; 2021 Aug; 64():101621. PubMed ID: 34371386
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Object permanence in young infants: further evidence.
    Baillargeon R; DeVos J
    Child Dev; 1991 Dec; 62(6):1227-46. PubMed ID: 1786712
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The development of calibration-based reasoning about collision events in young infants.
    Kotovsky L; Baillargeon R
    Cognition; 1998 Jul; 67(3):311-51. PubMed ID: 9775513
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Developmental changes in infant spatial categorization: when more is best and when less is enough.
    Casasola M; Park Y
    Child Dev; 2013; 84(3):1004-19. PubMed ID: 23163738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Negativity bias in infants' expectations about agents' dispositions.
    Chae JJK; Song HJ
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Nov; 36(4):620-633. PubMed ID: 29717497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Developments in young infants' reasoning about occluded objects.
    Aguiar A; Baillargeon R
    Cogn Psychol; 2002 Sep; 45(2):267-336. PubMed ID: 12528903
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Development of three-dimensional object completion in infancy.
    Soska KC; Johnson SP
    Child Dev; 2008; 79(5):1230-6. PubMed ID: 18826522
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.