These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

127 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 10986365)

  • 21. The use of trait labels in making psychological inferences.
    Heyman GD; Gelman SA
    Child Dev; 1999; 70(3):604-19. PubMed ID: 10368912
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning.
    Namy LL; Gentner D
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2002 Mar; 131(1):5-15. PubMed ID: 11900103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Children's predictions of consistency in people's actions.
    Kalish CW
    Cognition; 2002 Jul; 84(3):237-65. PubMed ID: 12044735
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Detecting blickets: how young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction.
    Gopnik A; Sobel DM
    Child Dev; 2000; 71(5):1205-22. PubMed ID: 11108092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Developmental changes within the core of artifact concepts.
    Matan A; Carey S
    Cognition; 2001 Jan; 78(1):1-26. PubMed ID: 11062320
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?
    Smith LB; Jones SS; Landau B
    Cognition; 1996 Aug; 60(2):143-71. PubMed ID: 8811743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Development of children's understanding of basic-subordinate inclusion relations.
    Johnson KE; Scott P; Mervis CB
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Sep; 33(5):745-63. PubMed ID: 9300208
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Two- and three-year-olds infer and reason about design intentions in order to categorize broken objects.
    Nelson DG; Holt MB; Egan LC
    Dev Sci; 2004 Nov; 7(5):543-9. PubMed ID: 15603287
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. When three is less than two: early developments in children's understanding of fractional quantities.
    Sophian C; Garyantes D; Chang C
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Sep; 33(5):731-44. PubMed ID: 9300207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. The differing roles of comparison and contrast in children's categorization.
    Namy LL; Clepper LE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):291-305. PubMed ID: 20609449
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Inductive generalization relies on category representations.
    Sutherland SL; Cimpian A
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2017 Apr; 24(2):632-636. PubMed ID: 26743059
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Children's understanding of illness: the generalization of illness according to exemplar.
    Buchanan-Barrow E; Barrett M; Bati M
    J Health Psychol; 2003 Nov; 8(6):659-70. PubMed ID: 14670201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. The absence of a shape bias in children's word learning.
    Cimpian A; Markman EM
    Dev Psychol; 2005 Nov; 41(6):1003-19. PubMed ID: 16351346
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. A developmental dissociation between category and function judgments about novel artifacts.
    Defeyter MA; Hearing J; German TC
    Cognition; 2009 Feb; 110(2):260-4. PubMed ID: 19101667
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. The role of motion in children's categorization of objects.
    Mak BS; Vera AH
    Cognition; 1999 May; 71(1):B11-21. PubMed ID: 10394711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Early noun lexicons in English and Japanese.
    Yoshida H; Smith LB
    Cognition; 2001 Dec; 82(2):B63-74. PubMed ID: 11716835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Priorities in children's expectations about object label reference: form over color.
    Baldwin DA
    Child Dev; 1989 Dec; 60(6):1291-306. PubMed ID: 2612242
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Conceptually coherent categories support label-based inductive generalization in preschoolers.
    Booth AE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Jul; 123():1-14. PubMed ID: 24632505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Learning higher-order generalizations through free play: Evidence from 2- and 3-year-old children.
    Sim ZL; Xu F
    Dev Psychol; 2017 Apr; 53(4):642-651. PubMed ID: 28333526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Robots and rodents: children's inferences about living and nonliving kinds.
    Jipson JL; Gelman SA
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(6):1675-88. PubMed ID: 17988314
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.