These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

238 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 11082861)

  • 1. The quality of false memory over time: is memory for misinformation "remembered" or "known"?
    Frost P
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2000 Sep; 7(3):531-6. PubMed ID: 11082861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Misled subjects may know more than their performance implies.
    Zaragoza MS; Koshmider JW
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 1989 Mar; 15(2):246-55. PubMed ID: 2522514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Why misinformation is more likely to be recognised over time: A source monitoring account.
    Frost P; Ingraham M; Wilson B
    Memory; 2002 May; 10(3):179-85. PubMed ID: 11958722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. When a lie becomes the truth: the effects of self-generated misinformation on eyewitness memory.
    Pickel KL
    Memory; 2004 Jan; 12(1):14-26. PubMed ID: 15098618
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. When Misinformation Improves Memory.
    Putnam AL; Sungkhasettee VW; Roediger HL
    Psychol Sci; 2017 Jan; 28(1):36-46. PubMed ID: 27879321
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The effects of age on remembering and knowing misinformation.
    Saunders J; Jess A
    Memory; 2010 Jan; 18(1):1-11. PubMed ID: 20013464
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Confidence-accuracy resolution in the misinformation paradigm is influenced by the availability of source cues.
    Horry R; Colton LM; Williamson P
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2014 Sep; 151():164-73. PubMed ID: 24983514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Trauma and memory: effects of post-event misinformation, retrieval order, and retention interval.
    Paz-Alonso PM; Goodman GS
    Memory; 2008 Jan; 16(1):58-75. PubMed ID: 17852727
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Influences of misleading postevent information: misinformation interference and acceptance.
    Belli RF
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 1989 Mar; 118(1):72-85. PubMed ID: 2522507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Assessment of calibration for reconstructed eye-witness memories.
    Bonham AJ; González-Vallejo C
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2009 May; 131(1):34-52. PubMed ID: 19362279
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Protecting memory from misinformation: Warnings modulate cortical reinstatement during memory retrieval.
    Karanian JM; Rabb N; Wulff AN; Torrance MG; Thomas AK; Race E
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2020 Sep; 117(37):22771-22779. PubMed ID: 32868423
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Repetition, not number of sources, increases both susceptibility to misinformation and confidence in the accuracy of eyewitnesses.
    Foster JL; Huthwaite T; Yesberg JA; Garry M; Loftus EF
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2012 Feb; 139(2):320-6. PubMed ID: 22257711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A comparison of the multimemory and detection theories of know and remember recognition judgments.
    Xu M; Bellezza FS
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2001 Sep; 27(5):1197-210. PubMed ID: 11550748
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Memory impairment in the weapon focus effect.
    Saunders J
    Mem Cognit; 2009 Apr; 37(3):326-35. PubMed ID: 19246347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The difference between implicit and explicit associative processes at study in creating false memory in the DRM paradigm.
    Kawasaki Y; Yama H
    Memory; 2006 Jan; 14(1):68-78. PubMed ID: 16423743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children's eyewitness memory: repeating post-event misinformation reduces the distinctiveness of a witnessed event.
    Bright-Paul A; Jarrold C
    Memory; 2012; 20(8):818-35. PubMed ID: 22963045
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The detection model of recognition using know and remember judgments.
    Inoue C; Bellezza FS
    Mem Cognit; 1998 Mar; 26(2):299-308. PubMed ID: 9584437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Misinformation effects in eyewitness memory: the presence and absence of memory impairment as a function of warning and misinformation accessibility.
    Eakin DK; Schreiber TA; Sergent-Marshall S
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2003 Sep; 29(5):813-25. PubMed ID: 14516215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Recalling a witnessed event increases eyewitness suggestibility: the reversed testing effect.
    Chan JC; Thomas AK; Bulevich JB
    Psychol Sci; 2009 Jan; 20(1):66-73. PubMed ID: 19037905
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Telling a good story: The effects of memory retrieval and context processing on eyewitness suggestibility.
    LaPaglia JA; Chan JCK
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(2):e0212592. PubMed ID: 30789952
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.