These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

124 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 11108096)

  • 1. Two-year-olds will name artifacts by their functions.
    Kemler Nelson DG; Russell R; Duke N; Jones K
    Child Dev; 2000; 71(5):1271-88. PubMed ID: 11108096
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The sources of young children's name innovations for novel artifacts.
    Nelson DG; Herron L; Holt MB
    J Child Lang; 2003 Nov; 30(4):823-43. PubMed ID: 14686086
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Young children's use of functional information to categorize artifacts: three factors that matter.
    Kemler Nelson DG; Frankenfield A; Morris C; Blair E
    Cognition; 2000 Nov; 77(2):133-68. PubMed ID: 10986365
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The role of comparison in preschoolers' novel object categorization.
    Graham SA; Namy LL; Gentner D; Meagher K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):280-90. PubMed ID: 20643266
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?
    Smith LB; Jones SS; Landau B
    Cognition; 1996 Aug; 60(2):143-71. PubMed ID: 8811743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Young children are sensitive to how an object was created when deciding what to name it.
    Gelman SA; Bloom P
    Cognition; 2000 Aug; 76(2):91-103. PubMed ID: 10856739
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Priming overgeneralizations in two- and four-year-old children.
    Gershkoff-Stowe L; Connell B; Smith L
    J Child Lang; 2006 Aug; 33(3):461-86. PubMed ID: 17017276
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Domain differences in the weights of perceptual and conceptual information in children's categorization.
    Diesendruck G; Peretz S
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Dec; 49(12):2383-95. PubMed ID: 23437802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. What's in a shape? Children represent shape variability differently than adults when naming objects.
    Abecassis M; Sera MD; Yonas A; Schwade J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Mar; 78(3):213-39. PubMed ID: 11222000
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Preschoolers' extension of novel words to animals and artifacts.
    Graham SA; Welder AN; Merrifield BA; Berman JM
    J Child Lang; 2010 Sep; 37(4):913-27. PubMed ID: 19874639
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Two- and three-year-olds infer and reason about design intentions in order to categorize broken objects.
    Nelson DG; Holt MB; Egan LC
    Dev Sci; 2004 Nov; 7(5):543-9. PubMed ID: 15603287
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects.
    Gelman SA; Taylor M
    Child Dev; 1984 Aug; 55(4):1535-40. PubMed ID: 6488963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. They call it like they see it: spontaneous naming and attention to shape.
    Samuelson LK; Smith LB
    Dev Sci; 2005 Mar; 8(2):182-98. PubMed ID: 15720376
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Detecting blickets: how young children use information about novel causal powers in categorization and induction.
    Gopnik A; Sobel DM
    Child Dev; 2000; 71(5):1205-22. PubMed ID: 11108092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Children's reliance on creator's intent in extending names for artifacts.
    Diesendruck G; Markson L; Bloom P
    Psychol Sci; 2003 Mar; 14(2):164-8. PubMed ID: 12661679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. It's a sign of the kind: gestures and words guide infants' inductive inferences.
    Graham SA; Kilbreath CS
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1111-23. PubMed ID: 17723039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. What can be learned from something's not being named.
    Merriman WE; Marazita JM; Jarvis LH; Evey-Burkey JA; Biggins M
    Child Dev; 1995 Dec; 66(6):1890-908. PubMed ID: 8556906
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Preschoolers fast map and retain artifact functions as efficiently as artifact names, but artifact actions are the most easily learned.
    Holland AK; Hyde G; Riggs KJ; Simpson A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Jun; 170():57-71. PubMed ID: 29432936
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Developmental differences in the naming of contextually non-categorical objects.
    Ozcan M
    J Psycholinguist Res; 2012 Feb; 41(1):51-69. PubMed ID: 21993900
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The use of linguistic and non-linguistic strategies in kindergarteners' interpretations of 'more' and 'less'.
    Hudson LM; Guthrie KH; Santilli NR
    J Child Lang; 1982 Feb; 9(1):125-38. PubMed ID: 7061624
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.