These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

186 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 11480935)

  • 21. Focusing on the relation: fewer exemplars facilitate children's initial verb learning and extension.
    Maguire MJ; Hirsh-Pasek K; Golinkoff RM; Brandone AC
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jul; 11(4):628-34. PubMed ID: 18576970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Number words in young children's conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion.
    Canobi KH; Bethune NE
    Cognition; 2008 Sep; 108(3):675-86. PubMed ID: 18621361
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Causal relations and feature similarity in children's inductive reasoning.
    Hayes BK; Thompson SP
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2007 Aug; 136(3):470-84. PubMed ID: 17696694
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Domain generality and specificity in children's causal inference about ambiguous data.
    Sobel DM; Munro SE
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Mar; 45(2):511-24. PubMed ID: 19271835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. The abstraction of form in semantic categories.
    Rubin DC; Stoltzfus ER; Wall KL
    Mem Cognit; 1991 Jan; 19(1):1-7. PubMed ID: 2017026
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Taking the task seriously: reflections on measures of color acquisition.
    Sandhofer CM; Thom EE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2006 Aug; 94(4):344-8. PubMed ID: 16714032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. On the relationship between category intensions and extensions in children.
    Caplan LJ; Barr RA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1989 Jun; 47(3):413-29. PubMed ID: 2738513
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Piecing together numerical language: children's use of default units in early counting and quantification.
    Brooks N; Pogue A; Barner D
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jan; 14(1):44-57. PubMed ID: 21159087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Implicit learning out of the lab: the case of orthographic regularities.
    Pacton S; Perruchet P; Fayol M; Cleeremans A
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2001 Sep; 130(3):401-26. PubMed ID: 11561917
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. BatMon II: children's category norms for 33 categories.
    Price HL; Connolly DA
    Behav Res Methods; 2006 Aug; 38(3):529-31. PubMed ID: 17186764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Cascading activation across levels of representation in children's lexical processing.
    Huang YT; Snedeker J
    J Child Lang; 2011 Jun; 38(3):644-61. PubMed ID: 20738890
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Naming in young children: a dumb attentional mechanism?
    Smith LB; Jones SS; Landau B
    Cognition; 1996 Aug; 60(2):143-71. PubMed ID: 8811743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts.
    Hollich G; Golinkoff RM; Hirsh-Pasek K
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1051-61. PubMed ID: 17723035
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Linear separability in superordinate natural language concepts.
    Ruts W; Storms G; Hampton J
    Mem Cognit; 2004 Jan; 32(1):83-95. PubMed ID: 15078046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. The development of attribute dominance in the knowledge base.
    Lindberg MA
    J Genet Psychol; 1989 Sep; 150(3):269-80. PubMed ID: 2809574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: links between preschoolers' theory of mind and semantic development.
    Sabbagh MA; Baldwin DA
    Child Dev; 2001; 72(4):1054-70. PubMed ID: 11480934
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Portuguese category norms for children.
    Carneiro P; Albuquerque P; Fernandez A
    Behav Res Methods; 2008 Feb; 40(1):177-82. PubMed ID: 18411540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Inconsistency with prior knowledge triggers children's causal explanatory reasoning.
    Legare CH; Gelman SA; Wellman HM
    Child Dev; 2010; 81(3):929-44. PubMed ID: 20573114
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Children's use of syntactic and pragmatic knowledge in the interpretation of novel adjectives.
    Diesendruck G; Hall DG; Graham SA
    Child Dev; 2006; 77(1):16-30. PubMed ID: 16460522
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Higher-order structure and relational reasoning: contrasting analogical and thematic relations.
    Goswami U; Brown AL
    Cognition; 1990 Sep; 36(3):207-26. PubMed ID: 2265527
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.