These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

884 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 11488378)

  • 1. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model.
    Usher M; McClelland JL
    Psychol Rev; 2001 Jul; 108(3):550-92. PubMed ID: 11488378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Extending a biologically inspired model of choice: multi-alternatives, nonlinearity and value-based multidimensional choice.
    Bogacz R; Usher M; Zhang J; McClelland JL
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2007 Sep; 362(1485):1655-70. PubMed ID: 17428774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. A memory-based model of Hick's law.
    Schneider DW; Anderson JR
    Cogn Psychol; 2011 May; 62(3):193-222. PubMed ID: 21293788
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Hick's law for choice reaction time: A review.
    Proctor RW; Schneider DW
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2018 Jun; 71(6):1281-1299. PubMed ID: 28434379
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Modeling 2-alternative forced-choice tasks: Accounting for both magnitude and difference effects.
    Ratcliff R; Voskuilen C; Teodorescu A
    Cogn Psychol; 2018 Jun; 103():1-22. PubMed ID: 29501775
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Context effects in multi-alternative decision making: empirical data and a Bayesian model.
    Hawkins G; Brown SD; Steyvers M; Wagenmakers EJ
    Cogn Sci; 2012 Apr; 36(3):498-516. PubMed ID: 22257112
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Implicit short-term memory and event frequency effects in visual search.
    Maljkovic V; Martini P
    Vision Res; 2005 Oct; 45(21):2831-46. PubMed ID: 16023172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Specific Relationship between the Shape of the Readiness Potential, Subjective Decision Time, and Waiting Time Predicted by an Accumulator Model with Temporally Autocorrelated Input Noise.
    Schurger A
    eNeuro; 2018; 5(1):. PubMed ID: 29464192
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. An integration-to-bound model of decision-making that accounts for the spectral properties of neural data.
    Guevara Erra R; Arbotto M; Schurger A
    Sci Rep; 2019 Jun; 9(1):8365. PubMed ID: 31182724
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Time course of free-choice priming effects explained by a simple accumulator model.
    Mattler U; Palmer S
    Cognition; 2012 Jun; 123(3):347-60. PubMed ID: 22475294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Dynamic integration of reward and stimulus information in perceptual decision-making.
    Gao J; Tortell R; McClelland JL
    PLoS One; 2011 Mar; 6(3):e16749. PubMed ID: 21390225
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Different (key)strokes for different folks: How standard and nonstandard typists balance Fitts' law and Hick's law.
    Logan GD; Ulrich JE; Lindsey DR
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2016 Dec; 42(12):2084-2102. PubMed ID: 27748613
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Accumulating advantages: A new conceptualization of rapid multiple choice.
    van Ravenzwaaij D; Brown SD; Marley AAJ; Heathcote A
    Psychol Rev; 2020 Mar; 127(2):186-215. PubMed ID: 31580104
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Choice reaction time in pigeons fails to increase as predicted by Hick's law.
    Flaim M; Guo J; Blaisdell AP
    Behav Processes; 2023 Mar; 206():104838. PubMed ID: 36716901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Unbounded evidence accumulation characterizes subjective visual vertical forced-choice perceptual choice and confidence.
    Lim K; Wang W; Merfeld DM
    J Neurophysiol; 2017 Nov; 118(5):2636-2653. PubMed ID: 28747465
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Similarity and number of alternatives in the random-dot motion paradigm.
    van Maanen L; Grasman RP; Forstmann BU; Keuken MC; Brown SD; Wagenmakers EJ
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2012 May; 74(4):739-53. PubMed ID: 22287207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Disentangling decision models: from independence to competition.
    Teodorescu AR; Usher M
    Psychol Rev; 2013 Jan; 120(1):1-38. PubMed ID: 23356779
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Perception and preference in short-term word priming.
    Huber DE; Shiffrin RM; Lyle KB; Ruys KI
    Psychol Rev; 2001 Jan; 108(1):149-82. PubMed ID: 11212626
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Multialternative drift-diffusion model predicts the relationship between visual fixations and choice in value-based decisions.
    Krajbich I; Rangel A
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2011 Aug; 108(33):13852-7. PubMed ID: 21808009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Optimal decision network with distributed representation.
    Bogacz R
    Neural Netw; 2007 Jul; 20(5):564-76. PubMed ID: 17399947
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 45.