These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
164 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 11706467)
1. Infants' reliance on shape to generalize novel labels to animate and inanimate objects. Graham SA; Poulin-Dubois D J Child Lang; 1999 Jun; 26(2):295-320. PubMed ID: 11706467 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants' inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties. Welder AN; Graham SA Child Dev; 2001; 72(6):1653-73. PubMed ID: 11768138 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Infants' ability to associate motion paths with object kinds. Baker RK; Pettigrew TL; Poulin-Dubois D Infant Behav Dev; 2014 Feb; 37(1):119-29. PubMed ID: 24486789 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Developing object concepts in infancy: an associative learning perspective. Rakison DH; Lupyan G Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2008; 73(1):vii, 1-110. PubMed ID: 18346226 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences. Graham SA; Kilbreath CS; Welder AN Child Dev; 2004; 75(2):409-27. PubMed ID: 15056196 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Word generalization by a dog (Canis familiaris): is shape important? van der Zee E; Zulch H; Mills D PLoS One; 2012; 7(11):e49382. PubMed ID: 23185321 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Rapid word learning in 13- and 17-month-olds in a naturalistic two-word procedure: looking versus reaching measures. Gurteen PM; Horne PJ; Erjavec M J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Jun; 109(2):201-17. PubMed ID: 21216414 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Infants transfer nonobvious properties from pictures to real-world objects. Keates J; Graham SA; Ganea PA J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Sep; 125():35-47. PubMed ID: 24814205 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. It's a sign of the kind: gestures and words guide infants' inductive inferences. Graham SA; Kilbreath CS Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1111-23. PubMed ID: 17723039 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Appreciating language conventions: thirteen-month-old Chinese infants understand that word generalization is shared practice. Liu S; Sun R J Child Lang; 2019 Jul; 46(4):812-823. PubMed ID: 31018878 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Do 9-month-old infants expect distinct words to refer to kinds? Dewar K; Xu F Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1227-38. PubMed ID: 17723047 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. What's in a shape? Children represent shape variability differently than adults when naming objects. Abecassis M; Sera MD; Yonas A; Schwade J J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Mar; 78(3):213-39. PubMed ID: 11222000 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. The absence of a shape bias in children's word learning. Cimpian A; Markman EM Dev Psychol; 2005 Nov; 41(6):1003-19. PubMed ID: 16351346 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. The effect of masks on infants' ability to fast-map and generalize new words. Liu S; Li X; Sun R J Child Lang; 2024 May; 51(3):637-655. PubMed ID: 38189211 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The role of symbol-based experience in early learning and transfer from pictures: evidence from Tanzania. Walker CM; Walker LB; Ganea PA Dev Psychol; 2013 Jul; 49(7):1315-24. PubMed ID: 22822932 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Who is the dynamic duo? How infants learn about the identity of objects in a causal chain. Rakison DH; Smith GT; Ali A Dev Psychol; 2016 Mar; 52(3):355-63. PubMed ID: 26689760 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]