These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
4. The role of sensory uncertainty in simple contour integration. Zhou Y; Acerbi L; Ma WJ PLoS Comput Biol; 2020 Nov; 16(11):e1006308. PubMed ID: 33253195 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Edge co-occurrence in natural images predicts contour grouping performance. Geisler WS; Perry JS; Super BJ; Gallogly DP Vision Res; 2001 Mar; 41(6):711-24. PubMed ID: 11248261 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. The role of shape complexity in the detection of closed contours. Wilder J; Feldman J; Singh M Vision Res; 2016 Sep; 126():220-231. PubMed ID: 26505685 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Directional harmonic theory: a computational Gestalt model to account for illusory contour and vertex formation. Lehar S Perception; 2003; 32(4):423-48. PubMed ID: 12785482 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Visual extrapolation of contour geometry. Singh M; Fulvio JM Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2005 Jan; 102(3):939-44. PubMed ID: 15647347 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. [The neural mechanism of visual contour integration]. Li Y; Wang YH; Li S Sheng Li Xue Bao; 2019 Feb; 71(1):45-52. PubMed ID: 30778503 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Does contour classification precede contour grouping in perception of partially visible figures? Scheessele MR; Pizlo Z Perception; 2007; 36(4):558-80. PubMed ID: 17564202 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Bayes and the simplicity principle in perception. Feldman J Psychol Rev; 2009 Oct; 116(4):875-887. PubMed ID: 19839688 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Perceptual attraction in tool use: evidence for a reliability-based weighting mechanism. Debats NB; Ernst MO; Heuer H J Neurophysiol; 2017 Apr; 117(4):1569-1580. PubMed ID: 28100656 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Ecological statistics of Gestalt laws for the perceptual organization of contours. Elder JH; Goldberg RM J Vis; 2002; 2(4):324-53. PubMed ID: 12678582 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Confidence judgments during ratio comparisons reveal a Bayesian bias. Alonso-Diaz S; Cantlon JF Cognition; 2018 Aug; 177():98-106. PubMed ID: 29656015 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. High-capacity preconscious processing in concurrent groupings of colored dots. Sun P; Chubb C; Wright CE; Sperling G Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2018 Dec; 115(52):E12153-E12162. PubMed ID: 30545909 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Decision-theoretic models of visual perception and action. Maloney LT; Zhang H Vision Res; 2010 Nov; 50(23):2362-74. PubMed ID: 20932856 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Contour integration with corners. Persike M; Meinhardt G Vision Res; 2016 Oct; 127():132-140. PubMed ID: 27542687 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Occlusion cues contribute to orientation judgments of occlusion-defined contours. De Weerd P; Vandenbussche E; Orban GA Percept Psychophys; 1993 Dec; 54(6):706-15. PubMed ID: 8134241 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. The global slowdown effect: why does perceptual grouping reduce perceived speed? Kohler PJ; Caplovitz GP; Tse PU Atten Percept Psychophys; 2014 Apr; 76(3):780-92. PubMed ID: 24448695 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. A Bayesian framework for cue integration in multistable grouping: Proximity, collinearity, and orientation priors in zigzag lattices. Claessens PM; Wagemans J J Vis; 2008 Dec; 8(7):33.1-23. PubMed ID: 19146265 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]