These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

213 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 12137134)

  • 21. Grouping by form in young infants: only relevant variability promotes perceptual learning.
    Quinn PC; Bhatt RS
    Perception; 2012; 41(12):1468-76. PubMed ID: 23586286
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Can chimpanzee infants (Pan troglodytes) form categorical representations in the same manner as human infants (Homo sapiens)?
    Murai C; Kosugi D; Tomonaga M; Tanaka M; Matsuzawa T; Itakura S
    Dev Sci; 2005 May; 8(3):240-54. PubMed ID: 15819756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. The Whole Warps the Sum of Its Parts.
    Corbett JE
    Psychol Sci; 2017 Jan; 28(1):12-22. PubMed ID: 27879322
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: II. Conceptual and theoretical foundations.
    Wagemans J; Feldman J; Gepshtein S; Kimchi R; Pomerantz JR; van der Helm PA; van Leeuwen C
    Psychol Bull; 2012 Nov; 138(6):1218-52. PubMed ID: 22845750
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Perceptual organization in schizophrenia: utilization of the Gestalt principles.
    Chey J; Holzman PS
    J Abnorm Psychol; 1997 Nov; 106(4):530-8. PubMed ID: 9358683
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The interplay between perceptual organization and categorization in the representation of complex visual patterns by young infants.
    Quinn PC; Schyns PG; Goldstone RL
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2006 Oct; 95(2):117-27. PubMed ID: 16797025
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. The influence of shape similarity and shared labels on infants' inductive inferences about nonobvious object properties.
    Welder AN; Graham SA
    Child Dev; 2001; 72(6):1653-73. PubMed ID: 11768138
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Is the asymmetry in young infants' categorization of humans versus nonhuman animals based on head, body, or global gestalt information?
    Quinn PC
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2004 Feb; 11(1):92-7. PubMed ID: 15116992
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Thirteen-month-olds rely on shared labels and shape similarity for inductive inferences.
    Graham SA; Kilbreath CS; Welder AN
    Child Dev; 2004; 75(2):409-27. PubMed ID: 15056196
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Long-term retention in 3.5-month-olds: familiarization time and individual differences in attentional style.
    Courage ML; Howe ML
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Jul; 79(3):271-93. PubMed ID: 11394930
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Ecological statistics of Gestalt laws for the perceptual organization of contours.
    Elder JH; Goldberg RM
    J Vis; 2002; 2(4):324-53. PubMed ID: 12678582
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Row blindness in Gestalt grouping and developmental dyslexia.
    Lewis JP; Frick RW
    Neuropsychologia; 1999 Mar; 37(3):385-93. PubMed ID: 10199650
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Neural markers of subordinate-level categorization in 6- to 7-month-old infants.
    Quinn PC; Doran MM; Reiss JE; Hoffman JE
    Dev Sci; 2010 May; 13(3):499-507. PubMed ID: 20443970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Learning perceptual organization in infancy: the effect of simultaneous versus sequential variability experience.
    Quinn PC; Bhatt RS
    Perception; 2010; 39(6):795-806. PubMed ID: 20698474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Connection-based and object-based grouping in multiple-object tracking: A developmental study.
    Van der Hallen R; Reusens J; Evers K; de-Wit L; Wagemans J
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Nov; 36(4):606-619. PubMed ID: 29600820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The role of shape recognition in figure/ground perception in infancy.
    White H; Jubran R; Heck A; Chroust A; Bhatt RS
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2018 Aug; 25(4):1381-1387. PubMed ID: 29713945
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Young infants' perception of unity and form in occlusion displays.
    Johnson SP; Bremner JG; Slater AM; Mason UC; Foster K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2002 Mar; 81(3):358-74. PubMed ID: 11884095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Perceptual development and category generalization.
    Smith LB
    Child Dev; 1979 Sep; 50(3):705-15. PubMed ID: 498847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Discrimination of possible and impossible objects in infancy.
    Shuwairi SM; Albert MK; Johnson SP
    Psychol Sci; 2007 Apr; 18(4):303-7. PubMed ID: 17470252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The perception of illusory transparent surfaces in infancy: early emergence of sensitivity to static pictorial cues.
    Otsuka Y; Yamazaki Y; Konishi Y; Kanazawa S; Yamaguchi MK; Spehar B
    J Vis; 2008 Dec; 8(16):6.1-12. PubMed ID: 19146272
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.