These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

131 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 12677334)

  • 1. Voluntary action expands perceived duration of its sensory consequence.
    Park J; Schlag-Rey M; Schlag J
    Exp Brain Res; 2003 Apr; 149(4):527-9. PubMed ID: 12677334
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Illusory perceptions of space and time preserve cross-saccadic perceptual continuity.
    Yarrow K; Haggard P; Heal R; Brown P; Rothwell JC
    Nature; 2001 Nov; 414(6861):302-5. PubMed ID: 11713528
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements.
    Melcher D; Kumar D; Srinivasan N
    Sci Rep; 2020 Oct; 10(1):16922. PubMed ID: 33037289
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Consistent chronostasis effects across saccade categories imply a subcortical efferent trigger.
    Yarrow K; Johnson H; Haggard P; Rothwell JC
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Jun; 16(5):839-47. PubMed ID: 15200711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Spatio-temporal contingency of saccade-induced chronostasis.
    Georg K; Lappe M
    Exp Brain Res; 2007 Jul; 180(3):535-9. PubMed ID: 17287990
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Sound reduces saccadic chronostasis illusion.
    Zhai M; Wu H; Wang Y; Liao Y; Feng W
    Vision Res; 2024 Feb; 215():108344. PubMed ID: 38109820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Spatio-temporal topography of saccadic overestimation of time.
    Knöll J; Morrone MC; Bremmer F
    Vision Res; 2013 May; 83():56-65. PubMed ID: 23458677
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Why are saccades influenced by the Brentano illusion?
    de Grave DD; Smeets JB; Brenner E
    Exp Brain Res; 2006 Oct; 175(1):177-82. PubMed ID: 16733700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action.
    Bruno N; Knox PC; de Grave DD
    Vision Res; 2010 Dec; 50(24):2671-82. PubMed ID: 20858510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Dynamic modulation of illusory and physical target size on separate and coordinated eye and hand movements.
    Gamble CM; Song JH
    J Vis; 2017 Mar; 17(3):23. PubMed ID: 28362898
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Perception of temporal duration affected by automatic and controlled movements.
    Herai T; Mogi K
    Conscious Cogn; 2014 Oct; 29():23-35. PubMed ID: 25108792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Spatial consequences of bridging the saccadic gap.
    Yarrow K; Whiteley L; Rothwell JC; Haggard P
    Vision Res; 2006 Feb; 46(4):545-55. PubMed ID: 16005489
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Chronostasis without voluntary action.
    Alexander I; Thilo KV; Cowey A; Walsh V
    Exp Brain Res; 2005 Feb; 161(1):125-32. PubMed ID: 15586278
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The influence of the Brentano illusion on eye and hand movements.
    de Grave DD; Franz VH; Gegenfurtner KR
    J Vis; 2006 Jun; 6(7):727-38. PubMed ID: 16895455
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements.
    Bahcall DO; Kowler E
    Nature; 1999 Aug; 400(6747):864-6. PubMed ID: 10476963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The effect of the Müller-Lyer configuration on saccadic eye movements is not fully due to illusory perception.
    Chen J; Yang P; Chen Z
    J Neurophysiol; 2020 Sep; 124(3):856-867. PubMed ID: 32783573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The hand knows something that the eye does not: reaching movements resist the Müller-Lyer illusion whether or not the target is foveated.
    Thompson AA; Westwood DA
    Neurosci Lett; 2007 Oct; 426(2):111-6. PubMed ID: 17890003
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Manual chronostasis: tactile perception precedes physical contact.
    Yarrow K; Rothwell JC
    Curr Biol; 2003 Jul; 13(13):1134-9. PubMed ID: 12842013
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Microsaccades drive illusory motion in the Enigma illusion.
    Troncoso XG; Macknik SL; Otero-Millan J; Martinez-Conde S
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2008 Oct; 105(41):16033-8. PubMed ID: 18843109
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The effects of illusory line motion on incongruent saccades: implications for saccadic eye movements and visual attention.
    Crawford TJ; Kean M; Klein RM; Hamm JP
    Exp Brain Res; 2006 Aug; 173(3):498-506. PubMed ID: 16528498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.