These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

164 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 14552409)

  • 1. Theory of mind and relational complexity.
    Andrews G; Halford GS; Bunch KM; Bowden D; Jones T
    Child Dev; 2003; 74(5):1476-99. PubMed ID: 14552409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Hindsight bias and developing theories of mind.
    Bernstein DM; Atance C; Meltzoff AN; Loftus GR
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(4):1374-94. PubMed ID: 17650144
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind.
    Carlson SM; Moses LJ
    Child Dev; 2001; 72(4):1032-53. PubMed ID: 11480933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Children's understanding of pictorial and mental representations.
    Slaughter V
    Child Dev; 1998 Apr; 69(2):321-32. PubMed ID: 9586208
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Preschoolers' use of desires to solve theory of mind problems in a pretense context.
    Cassidy KW
    Dev Psychol; 1998 May; 34(3):503-11. PubMed ID: 9597360
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Preschoolers' implicit and explicit false-belief understanding: relations with complex syntactical mastery.
    Low J
    Child Dev; 2010; 81(2):597-615. PubMed ID: 20438463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Young children's performance on the balance scale: the influence of relational complexity.
    Halford GS; Andrews G; Dalton C; Boag C; Zielinski T
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2002 Apr; 81(4):417-45. PubMed ID: 11890729
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Implicit and explicit false belief development in preschool children.
    Grosse Wiesmann C; Friederici AD; Singer T; Steinbeis N
    Dev Sci; 2017 Sep; 20(5):. PubMed ID: 27696613
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. True or false: do 5-year-olds understand belief?
    Fabricius WV; Boyer TW; Weimer AA; Carroll K
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Nov; 46(6):1402-16. PubMed ID: 21058830
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. When representations conflict with reality: the preschooler's problem with false beliefs and "false" photographs.
    Zaitchik D
    Cognition; 1990 Apr; 35(1):41-68. PubMed ID: 2340712
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Theory of Mind "emotion", developmental characteristics and social understanding in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities.
    Thirion-Marissiaux AF; Nader-Grosbois N
    Res Dev Disabil; 2008; 29(5):414-30. PubMed ID: 17855050
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Training transfer between card sorting and false belief understanding: helping children apply conflicting descriptions.
    Kloo D; Perner J
    Child Dev; 2003; 74(6):1823-39. PubMed ID: 14669898
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Domain-specificity and theory of mind: evaluating neuropsychological evidence.
    Apperly IA; Samson D; Humphreys GW
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2005 Dec; 9(12):572-7. PubMed ID: 16271505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. 2.5-year-olds succeed at a verbal anticipatory-looking false-belief task.
    He Z; Bolz M; Baillargeon R
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):14-29. PubMed ID: 22429030
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The role of control functions in mentalizing: dual-task studies of theory of mind and executive function.
    Bull R; Phillips LH; Conway CA
    Cognition; 2008 May; 107(2):663-72. PubMed ID: 17765214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development.
    Andrews G; Halford GS
    Cogn Psychol; 2002 Sep; 45(2):153-219. PubMed ID: 12528901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Not just a sum of its parts: How tasks of the theory of mind scale relate to executive function across time.
    Doenyas C; Yavuz HM; Selcuk B
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Feb; 166():485-501. PubMed ID: 29078144
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Forgetting common ground: six- to seven-year-olds have an overinterpretive theory of mind.
    Lagattuta KH; Sayfan L; Blattman AJ
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Nov; 46(6):1417-32. PubMed ID: 21058831
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning.
    Durrleman S; Franck J
    J Commun Disord; 2015; 54():15-31. PubMed ID: 25637130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific language impairment.
    Miller C
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2004; 39(2):191-213. PubMed ID: 15204451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.