These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
566 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 14585019)
1. Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Lamy D; Egeth HE J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Oct; 29(5):1003-20. PubMed ID: 14585019 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting. Muller HJ; Reimann B; Krummenacher J J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Oct; 29(5):1021-35. PubMed ID: 14585020 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. A search order lost effect: ignoring a singleton distractor affects visual search efficiency. Kumada T Vision Res; 2010 Jun; 50(14):1402-13. PubMed ID: 20025896 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. A static color discontinuity can capture spatial attention when the target is an abrupt-onset singleton. Burnham BR; Neely JH J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2008 Aug; 34(4):831-41. PubMed ID: 18665729 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Irrelevant singletons in visual search do not capture attention but can produce nonspatial filtering costs. Wykowska A; Schubö A J Cogn Neurosci; 2011 Mar; 23(3):645-60. PubMed ID: 19929330 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The active inhibition for the processing of visual irrelevant conflict information. Mao W; Wang Y Int J Psychophysiol; 2008 Jan; 67(1):47-53. PubMed ID: 17999937 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. The role of within-dimension singleton priming in visual search. Lamy D; Bar-Anan Y; Egeth HE J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2008 Apr; 34(2):268-85. PubMed ID: 18377170 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction by singletons: target uncertainty does. Lamy D; Yashar A Vision Res; 2008 May; 48(10):1274-9. PubMed ID: 18395767 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Processing multidimensional objects under different perceptual loads: the priority of bottom-up perceptual saliency. Wei P; Zhou X Brain Res; 2006 Oct; 1114(1):113-24. PubMed ID: 16935270 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Selecting and ignoring salient objects within and across dimensions in visual search. Schubö A; Müller HJ Brain Res; 2009 Aug; 1283():84-101. PubMed ID: 19501066 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Attentional capture by a salient distractor in visual search: the effect of target-distractor similarity. Poiese P; Spalek TM; Di Lollo V Can J Exp Psychol; 2008 Dec; 62(4):233-6. PubMed ID: 19071990 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The role of target-distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Becker SI J Exp Psychol Gen; 2010 May; 139(2):247-65. PubMed ID: 20438251 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Sources of top-down control in visual search. Weidner R; Krummenacher J; Reimann B; Müller HJ; Fink GR J Cogn Neurosci; 2009 Nov; 21(11):2100-13. PubMed ID: 19199412 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]