These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
305 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 14680774)
1. No evidence for sequential effects of the interaction of stereo and motion cues in judgements of perceived shape. Champion RA; Brenner E; Mamassian P; Simmons DR Vision Res; 2004 Mar; 44(5):483-7. PubMed ID: 14680774 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Effects of head motion and stereo viewing on perceived glossiness. Sakano Y; Ando H J Vis; 2010 Nov; 10(9):15. PubMed ID: 21106677 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Isotropic integration of binocular disparity and relative motion in the perception of three-dimensional shape. Hibbard PB; Bradshaw MF Spat Vis; 2002; 15(2):205-17. PubMed ID: 11991575 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Disparity-defined objects moving in depth do not elicit three-dimensional shape constancy. Scarfe P; Hibbard PB Vision Res; 2006 May; 46(10):1599-610. PubMed ID: 16364392 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Stereo and motion parallax cues in human 3D vision: can they vanish without a trace? Rauschecker AM; Solomon SG; Glennerster A J Vis; 2006 Dec; 6(12):1471-85. PubMed ID: 17209749 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Perception of scene-relative object movement: Optic flow parsing and the contribution of monocular depth cues. Warren PA; Rushton SK Vision Res; 2009 May; 49(11):1406-19. PubMed ID: 19480063 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Dichoptic motion perception limited to depth of fixation? Lages M; Dolia A; Graf EW Vision Res; 2007 Jan; 47(2):244-52. PubMed ID: 17112562 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. The task-dependent use of binocular disparity and motion parallax information. Bradshaw MF; Parton AD; Glennerster A Vision Res; 2000; 40(27):3725-34. PubMed ID: 11090665 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. The role of stereopsis, motion parallax, perspective and angle polarity in perceiving 3-D shape. Sherman A; Papathomas TV; Jain A; Keane BP Seeing Perceiving; 2012; 25(3-4):263-85. PubMed ID: 21771395 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Depth generalization from stereo to motion parallax in the owl. van der Willigen RF; Frost BJ; Wagner H J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol; 2002 Jan; 187(12):997-1007. PubMed ID: 11913818 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The interaction of binocular disparity and motion parallax in the computation of depth. Bradshaw MF; Rogers BJ Vision Res; 1996 Nov; 36(21):3457-68. PubMed ID: 8977012 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Do visual cues contribute to the neural estimate of viewing distance used by the oculomotor system? Wei M; DeAngelis GC; Angelaki DE J Neurosci; 2003 Sep; 23(23):8340-50. PubMed ID: 12967996 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Integration of stereopsis and motion shape cues. Johnston EB; Cumming BG; Landy MS Vision Res; 1994 Sep; 34(17):2259-75. PubMed ID: 7941420 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. A new method for assessing motion-in-depth perception in strabismic patients. Watanabe Y; Kezuka T; Harasawa K; Usui M; Yaguchi H; Shioiri S Br J Ophthalmol; 2008 Jan; 92(1):47-50. PubMed ID: 17596334 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. The intrinsic constraint model for stereo-motion integration. Tassinari H; Domini F Perception; 2008; 37(1):79-95. PubMed ID: 18399249 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Representation of shapes, edges, and surfaces across multiple cues in the human visual cortex. Vinberg J; Grill-Spector K J Neurophysiol; 2008 Mar; 99(3):1380-93. PubMed ID: 18171705 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Interaction between the perceived shape of two objects. Brenner E; Landy MS Vision Res; 1999 Nov; 39(23):3834-48. PubMed ID: 10748919 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]