These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

330 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 15549502)

  • 41. Spontaneous cross-species imitation in interactions between chimpanzees and zoo visitors.
    Persson T; Sauciuc GA; Madsen EA
    Primates; 2018 Jan; 59(1):19-29. PubMed ID: 28815382
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Inoue-Nakamura N; Matsuzawa T
    J Comp Psychol; 1997 Jun; 111(2):159-73. PubMed ID: 9170281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. What young chimpanzees know about seeing.
    Povinelli DJ; Eddy TJ
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1996; 61(3):i-vi, 1-152; discussion 153-91. PubMed ID: 8795292
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Social Learning in the Real-World: 'Over-Imitation' Occurs in Both Children and Adults Unaware of Participation in an Experiment and Independently of Social Interaction.
    Whiten A; Allan G; Devlin S; Kseib N; Raw N; McGuigan N
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(7):e0159920. PubMed ID: 27466806
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task.
    Gruber T; Muller MN; Strimling P; Wrangham R; Zuberbühler K
    Curr Biol; 2009 Nov; 19(21):1806-10. PubMed ID: 19853447
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique.
    Yamamoto S; Humle T; Tanaka M
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(1):e55768. PubMed ID: 23383278
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens).
    Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Jun; 115(2):159-71. PubMed ID: 11459163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Testing differential use of payoff-biased social learning strategies in children and chimpanzees.
    Vale GL; Flynn EG; Kendal J; Rawlings B; Hopper LM; Schapiro SJ; Lambeth SP; Kendal RL
    Proc Biol Sci; 2017 Dec; 284(1868):. PubMed ID: 29187629
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Preliminary assessment of methods used to demonstrate nut-cracking behavior to five captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Ross SR; Milstein MS; Calcutt SE; Lonsdorf EV
    Folia Primatol (Basel); 2010; 81(4):224-32. PubMed ID: 21124032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Myowa-Yamakoshi M; Matsuzawa T
    J Comp Psychol; 1999 Jun; 113(2):128-36. PubMed ID: 10384721
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Imitation in young children: when who gets copied is more important than what gets copied.
    Nielsen M; Blank C
    Dev Psychol; 2011 Jul; 47(4):1050-3. PubMed ID: 21639617
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Performance in a tool-using task by common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), bonobos (Pan paniscus), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Visalberghi E; Fragaszy DM; Savage-Rumbaugh S
    J Comp Psychol; 1995 Mar; 109(1):52-60. PubMed ID: 7705062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Comprehension of cause-effect relations in a tool-using task by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Limongelli L; Boysen ST; Visalberghi E
    J Comp Psychol; 1995 Mar; 109(1):18-26. PubMed ID: 7705056
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Evidence for a dual-process account of over-imitation: Children imitate anti- and prosocial models equally, but prefer prosocial models once they become aware of multiple solutions to a task.
    Schleihauf H; Hoehl S
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(9):e0256614. PubMed ID: 34529702
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Comparing humans and nonhuman great apes in the broken cloth problem: Is their knowledge causal or perceptual?
    Albiach-Serrano A; Sebastián-Enesco C; Seed A; Colmenares F; Call J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Nov; 139():174-89. PubMed ID: 26117496
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Naive chimpanzees' (Pan troglodytes) observation of experienced conspecifics in a tool-using task.
    Hirata S; Morimura N
    J Comp Psychol; 2000 Sep; 114(3):291-6. PubMed ID: 10994845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Testing the individual and social learning abilities of task-naïve captive chimpanzees (
    Neadle D; Bandini E; Tennie C
    PeerJ; 2020; 8():e8734. PubMed ID: 32195057
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Enculturated chimpanzees imitate rationally.
    Buttelmann D; Carpenter M; Call J; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2007 Jul; 10(4):F31-8. PubMed ID: 17552931
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Myowa-Yamakoshi M; Tomonaga M; Tanaka M; Matsuzawa T
    Dev Sci; 2004 Sep; 7(4):437-42. PubMed ID: 15484592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Exploring the evolutionary origins of overimitation: a comparison across domesticated and non-domesticated canids.
    Johnston AM; Holden PC; Santos LR
    Dev Sci; 2017 Jul; 20(4):. PubMed ID: 27659592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 17.