These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

173 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 15603284)

  • 1. The role of joint attention in the development of infants' play with objects.
    Bigelow AE; MacLean K; Proctor J
    Dev Sci; 2004 Nov; 7(5):518-26. PubMed ID: 15603284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Individual and relational contributions to parallel and joint attention in infancy.
    Osório A; Martins C; Meins E; Martins EC; Soares I
    Infant Behav Dev; 2011 Dec; 34(4):515-24. PubMed ID: 21868099
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Infants' visual sustained attention is higher during joint play than solo play: is this due to increased endogenous attention control or exogenous stimulus capture?
    Wass SV; Clackson K; Georgieva SD; Brightman L; Nutbrown R; Leong V
    Dev Sci; 2018 Nov; 21(6):e12667. PubMed ID: 29624833
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Joint attention at 10 months of age in infant-mother dyads: contrasting free toy-play with semi-structured toy-play.
    Mateus V; Martins C; Osório A; Martins EC; Soares I
    Infant Behav Dev; 2013 Feb; 36(1):176-9. PubMed ID: 23254120
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Infants, mothers, and dyadic contributions to stability and prediction of social stress response at 6 months.
    Provenzi L; Olson KL; Montirosso R; Tronick E
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Jan; 52(1):1-8. PubMed ID: 26569560
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Mothers' insightfulness regarding their infants' internal experience: relations with maternal sensitivity and infant attachment.
    Koren-Karie N; Oppenheim D; Dolev S; Sher E; Etzion-Carasso A
    Dev Psychol; 2002 Jul; 38(4):534-42. PubMed ID: 12090483
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Maternal sensitivity throughout infancy: continuity and relation to attachment security.
    Bigelow AE; MacLean K; Proctor J; Myatt T; Gillis R; Power M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Feb; 33(1):50-60. PubMed ID: 20004022
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Number and type of toys affect joint attention of mothers and infants.
    Koşkulu S; Küntay AC; Liszkowski U; Uzundag BA
    Infant Behav Dev; 2021 Aug; 64():101589. PubMed ID: 34058634
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Infants' interactions with mother, sibling, and peer: contrasts and relations between interaction systems.
    Vandell DL; Wilson KS
    Child Dev; 1987 Feb; 58(1):176-86. PubMed ID: 3816342
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Developmental changes in infants' and mothers' pathways to achieving joint attention episodes.
    Loy M; Masur EF; Olson J
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Feb; 50():264-273. PubMed ID: 29462746
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Previous manipulative experience in relation to infants' processing of mothers' presentations of objects.
    Demetre JD
    Percept Mot Skills; 1983 Oct; 57(2):484-6. PubMed ID: 6634330
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The impact of maternal gaze responsiveness on infants' gaze following and later vocabulary development.
    Wildt E; Rohlfing KJ
    Infant Behav Dev; 2024 Mar; 74():101917. PubMed ID: 38134835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Infant physiological response to the still-face paradigm: contributions of maternal sensitivity and infants' early regulatory behavior.
    Conradt E; Ablow J
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Jun; 33(3):251-65. PubMed ID: 20207419
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Dyadic co-regulation, affective intensity and infant's development at 12 months: A comparison among extremely preterm and full-term dyads.
    Sansavini A; Zavagli V; Guarini A; Savini S; Alessandroni R; Faldella G
    Infant Behav Dev; 2015 Aug; 40():29-40. PubMed ID: 26021805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Perturbation of Japanese mother-infant habitual interactions in the double video paradigm and relationship to maternal playfulness.
    Nakano S; Kondo-Ikemura K; Kusanagi E
    Infant Behav Dev; 2007 May; 30(2):213-31. PubMed ID: 17382400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Development of mother-infant interaction in tickling play: The relationship between infants' ticklishness and social behaviors.
    Ishijima K; Negayama K
    Infant Behav Dev; 2017 Nov; 49():161-167. PubMed ID: 28934614
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Longitudinal relations among maternal depressive symptoms, maternal mind-mindedness, and infant attachment behavior.
    Bigelow AE; Beebe B; Power M; Stafford AL; Ewing J; Egleson A; Kaminer T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 May; 51():33-44. PubMed ID: 29567547
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Neonatal Risk, Maternal Sensitive-Responsiveness and Infants' Joint Attention: Moderation by Stressful Contexts.
    Egotubov A; Atzaba-Poria N; Meiri G; Marks K; Gueron-Sela N
    J Abnorm Child Psychol; 2020 Mar; 48(3):453-466. PubMed ID: 31823166
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction.
    Bakeman R; Adamson LB
    Child Dev; 1984 Aug; 55(4):1278-89. PubMed ID: 6488956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Mother-infant joint attention and sharing: relations to disorganized attachment and maternal disrupted communication.
    Annie Yoon S; Kelso GA; Lock A; Lyons-Ruth K
    J Genet Psychol; 2014; 175(5-6):494-510. PubMed ID: 25412198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.