These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

211 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 15824938)

  • 21. Visual preference by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) for photos of primates measured by a free choice-order task: implication for influence of social experience.
    Tanaka M
    Primates; 2003 Apr; 44(2):157-65. PubMed ID: 12687480
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Chimpanzees play the ultimatum game.
    Proctor D; Williamson RA; de Waal FB; Brosnan SF
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2013 Feb; 110(6):2070-5. PubMed ID: 23319633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Macaque monkeys can learn token values from human models through vicarious reward.
    Bevacqua S; Cerasti E; Falcone R; Cervelloni M; Brunamonti E; Ferraina S; Genovesio A
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(3):e59961. PubMed ID: 23544115
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Do capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use tokens as symbols?
    Addessi E; Crescimbene L; Visalberghi E
    Proc Biol Sci; 2007 Oct; 274(1625):2579-85. PubMed ID: 17698487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Non-human primate token use shows possibilities but also limitations for establishing a form of currency.
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190675. PubMed ID: 33423633
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Language-naive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) judge relations between relations in a conceptual matching-to-sample task.
    Thompson RK; Oden DL; Boysen ST
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1997 Jan; 23(1):31-43. PubMed ID: 9008861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Spread of arbitrary conventions among chimpanzees: a controlled experiment.
    Bonnie KE; Horner V; Whiten A; de Waal FB
    Proc Biol Sci; 2007 Feb; 274(1608):367-72. PubMed ID: 17164200
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Token transfer between mother and offspring chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): mother-offspring interaction in a competitive situation.
    Tanaka M; Yamamoto S
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Oct; 12 Suppl 1():S19-26. PubMed ID: 19685088
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Trading up: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) show self-control through their exchange behavior.
    Beran MJ; Rossettie MS; Parrish AE
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Jan; 19(1):109-21. PubMed ID: 26325355
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Strategic navigation of two-dimensional alley mazes: comparing capuchin monkeys and chimpanzees.
    Fragaszy D; Johnson-Pynn J; Hirsh E; Brakke K
    Anim Cogn; 2003 Sep; 6(3):149-60. PubMed ID: 12955584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) fail a what-where-when task but find rewards by using a location-based association strategy.
    Dekleva M; Dufour V; de Vries H; Spruijt BM; Sterck EH
    PLoS One; 2011 Feb; 6(2):e16593. PubMed ID: 21359204
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Selfish strategies develop in social problem situations in chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) mother–infant pairs.
    Yamamoto S; Tanaka M
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Oct; 12 Suppl 1():27-36. PubMed ID: 19727865
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. When given the opportunity, chimpanzees maximize personal gain rather than "level the playing field".
    Hopper LM; Lambeth SP; Schapiro SJ; Brosnan SF
    PeerJ; 2013; 1():e165. PubMed ID: 24109550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Social comparison mediates chimpanzees' responses to loss, not frustration.
    Hopper LM; Lambeth SP; Schapiro SJ; Brosnan SF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1303-11. PubMed ID: 24880642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Chimpanzees' understanding of social leverage.
    Sánchez-Amaro A; Duguid S; Call J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(12):e0207868. PubMed ID: 30540763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Adult cleaner wrasse outperform capuchin monkeys, chimpanzees and orang-utans in a complex foraging task derived from cleaner--client reef fish cooperation.
    Salwiczek LH; Prétôt L; Demarta L; Proctor D; Essler J; Pinto AI; Wismer S; Stoinski T; Brosnan SF; Bshary R
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(11):e49068. PubMed ID: 23185293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Spontaneous prosocial choice by chimpanzees.
    Horner V; Carter JD; Suchak M; de Waal FB
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2011 Aug; 108(33):13847-51. PubMed ID: 21825175
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Acquisition of object-robbing and object/food-bartering behaviours: a culturally maintained token economy in free-ranging long-tailed macaques.
    Leca JB; Gunst N; Gardiner M; Wandia IN
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Mar; 376(1819):20190677. PubMed ID: 33423623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Bonobos and orangutans, but not chimpanzees, flexibly plan for the future in a token-exchange task.
    Bourjade M; Call J; Pelé M; Maumy M; Dufour V
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1329-40. PubMed ID: 24942106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Gambling primates: reactions to a modified Iowa Gambling Task in humans, chimpanzees and capuchin monkeys.
    Proctor D; Williamson RA; Latzman RD; de Waal FB; Brosnan SF
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Jul; 17(4):983-95. PubMed ID: 24504555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.