BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 15883353)

  • 1. Line bisection and rebisection: the crossover effect of space location.
    Wang Q; Sonoda S; Hanamura M; Okazaki H; Saitoh E
    Neurorehabil Neural Repair; 2005 Jun; 19(2):84-92. PubMed ID: 15883353
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Significance of paradoxical leftward error of line bisection in left unilateral spatial neglect.
    Ishiai S; Koyama Y; Seki K
    Brain Cogn; 2001 Mar; 45(2):238-48. PubMed ID: 11237369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Setting the midpoint of sentences: The role of the left hemisphere.
    Veronelli L; Arduino LS; Biscaro V; Corbo M; Vallar G
    Neuropsychologia; 2020 Feb; 137():107287. PubMed ID: 31863803
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Crossover by line length and spatial location.
    Mennemeier M; Rapcsak SZ; Pierce C; Vezey E
    Brain Cogn; 2001 Dec; 47(3):412-22. PubMed ID: 11748897
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Context and crossover in unilateral neglect.
    Ricci R; Chatterjee A
    Neuropsychologia; 2001; 39(11):1138-43. PubMed ID: 11527550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Representational neglect for words as revealed by bisection tasks.
    Arduino LS; Marinelli CV; Pasotti F; Ferrè ER; Bottini G
    J Neuropsychol; 2012 Mar; 6(1):43-64. PubMed ID: 22257574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Image of a line is not shrunk but neglected. Absence of crossover in unilateral spatial neglect.
    Ishiai S; Koyama Y; Nakano N; Seki K; Nishida Y; Hayashi K
    Neuropsychologia; 2004; 42(2):251-6. PubMed ID: 14644110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Spatial Neglect Predicts Upper Limb Use in the Activities of Daily Living.
    Vanbellingen T; Ottiger B; Maaijwee N; Pflugshaupt T; Bohlhalter S; Müri RM; Nef T; Cazzoli D; Nyffeler T
    Cerebrovasc Dis; 2017; 44(3-4):122-127. PubMed ID: 28605738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Causes of cross-over in unilateral neglect: between-group comparisons, within-patient dissociations and eye movements.
    Doricchi F; Guariglia P; Figliozzi F; Silvetti M; Bruno G; Gasparini M
    Brain; 2005 Jun; 128(Pt 6):1386-406. PubMed ID: 15758037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Rightward and leftward bisection biases in spatial neglect: two sides of the same coin?
    Savazzi S; Posteraro L; Veronesi G; Mancini F
    Brain; 2007 Aug; 130(Pt 8):2070-84. PubMed ID: 17584772
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Eye movement and online bisection task in unilateral patients with neglect: a new look to the 'gradient effect'.
    Balconi M; Amenta S; Sozzi M; Cannatà AP; Pisani L
    Brain Inj; 2013; 27(3):310-7. PubMed ID: 23438350
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The Character-line Bisection Task: a new test for hemispatial neglect.
    Lee BH; Kang SJ; Park JM; Son Y; Lee KH; Adair JC; Heilman KM; Na DL
    Neuropsychologia; 2004; 42(12):1715-24. PubMed ID: 15327939
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Unilateral spatial neglect in AD: significance of line bisection performance.
    Ishiai S; Koyama Y; Seki K; Orimo S; Sodeyama N; Ozawa E; Lee EY; Takahashi M; Watabiki S; Okiyama R; Ohtake T; Hiroki M
    Neurology; 2000 Aug; 55(3):364-70. PubMed ID: 10932269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Neglect-associated constructional disorders: a paradoxical phenomenon?
    Grossi D; Lepore M; Esposito A; Napolitano A; Serino M; Trojano L
    Neuropsychologia; 1999 May; 37(5):589-94. PubMed ID: 10340317
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Crossover is not a consequence of neglect: a test of the orientation/estimation hypothesis.
    Mennemeier M; Vezey E; Lamar M; Jewell G
    J Int Neuropsychol Soc; 2002 Jan; 8(1):107-14. PubMed ID: 11843067
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Biases in attentional orientation and magnitude estimation explain crossover: neglect is a disorder of both.
    Mennemeier M; Pierce CA; Chatterjee A; Anderson B; Jewell G; Dowler R; Woods AJ; Glenn T; Mark VW
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2005 Aug; 17(8):1194-211. PubMed ID: 16197678
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Space representation in children with dyslexia and children without dyslexia: contribution of line bisection and circle centering tasks.
    Vieira S; Quercia P; Bonnetblanc F; Michel C
    Res Dev Disabil; 2013 Nov; 34(11):3997-4008. PubMed ID: 24036389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Line versus representational bisections in unilateral spatial neglect.
    Ishiai S; Koyama Y; Seki K; Izawa M
    J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry; 2000 Dec; 69(6):745-50. PubMed ID: 11080226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. A battery of tests for the quantitative assessment of unilateral neglect.
    Azouvi P; Bartolomeo P; Beis JM; Perennou D; Pradat-Diehl P; Rousseaux M
    Restor Neurol Neurosci; 2006; 24(4-6):273-85. PubMed ID: 17119304
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Stimulus context in hemineglect.
    Marshall RS; Lazar RM; Krakauer JW; Sharma R
    Brain; 1998 Oct; 121 ( Pt 10)():2003-10. PubMed ID: 9798752
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.