These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
6. Spatial properties of disparity pooling in human stereo vision. Parker AJ; Yang Y Vision Res; 1989; 29(11):1525-38. PubMed ID: 2635478 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. The perceived visual direction of monocular objects in random-dot stereograms is influenced by perceived depth and allelotropia. Hariharan-Vilupuru S; Bedell HE Vision Res; 2009 Jan; 49(2):190-201. PubMed ID: 18992271 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Perceptual latency and complex random-dot stereograms. Bradshaw MF; Rogers BJ; De Bruyn B Perception; 1995; 24(7):749-59. PubMed ID: 8710438 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Poor visibility of motion in depth is due to early motion averaging. Harris JM; Rushton SK Vision Res; 2003 Feb; 43(4):385-92. PubMed ID: 12535995 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Differences in perceived depth for temporally correlated and uncorrelated dynamic random-dot stereograms. Gheorghiu E; Erkelens CJ Vision Res; 2005 Jun; 45(12):1603-14. PubMed ID: 15781076 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Effect of decorrelation on 3-D grating detection with static and dynamic random-dot stereograms. Palmisano S; Allison RS; Howard IP Vision Res; 2006 Jan; 46(1-2):57-71. PubMed ID: 16307771 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Learning to see random-dot stereograms. O'Toole AJ; Kersten DJ Perception; 1992; 21(2):227-43. PubMed ID: 1513672 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]