These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

102 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16271858)

  • 1. Decision making: the virtue of patience in primates.
    Long A; Platt M
    Curr Biol; 2005 Nov; 15(21):R874-6. PubMed ID: 16271858
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys.
    Stevens JR; Hallinan EV; Hauser MD
    Biol Lett; 2005 Jun; 1(2):223-6. PubMed ID: 17148172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The win-stay rule in foraging decisions by free-ranging titi monkeys (Callicebus cupreus cupreus) and Tamarins (Saguinus imperator imperator and Saguinus fuscicollis weddelli).
    Bicca-Marques JC
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 Aug; 119(3):343-51. PubMed ID: 16131263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Will travel for food: spatial discounting in two new world monkeys.
    Stevens JR; Rosati AG; Ross KR; Hauser MD
    Curr Biol; 2005 Oct; 15(20):1855-60. PubMed ID: 16243033
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Experimental evidence for route integration and strategic planning in wild capuchin monkeys.
    Janson CH
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jul; 10(3):341-56. PubMed ID: 17464518
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Comparative approaches to studying strategy: towards an evolutionary account of primate decision making.
    Brosnan SF; Beran MJ; Parrish AE; Price SA; Wilson BJ
    Evol Psychol; 2013 Jul; 11(3):606-27. PubMed ID: 23864296
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Use of spatial, visual, and olfactory information during foraging in wild nocturnal and diurnal anthropoids: A field experiment comparing Aotus, Callicebus, and Saguinus.
    Bicca-Marques JC; Garber PA
    Am J Primatol; 2004 Mar; 62(3):171-87. PubMed ID: 15027091
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Intolerance of uncertainty and decisions about delayed, probabilistic rewards.
    Luhmann CC; Ishida K; Hajcak G
    Behav Ther; 2011 Sep; 42(3):378-86. PubMed ID: 21658521
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. [Some evolutionary aspects of group food-seeking behavior in phylogenesis of primates].
    Maliukova IV; Uvarova IA; Molotova NG; Nikitin VS
    Zh Evol Biokhim Fiziol; 2003; 39(6):568-76. PubMed ID: 14983686
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Reward processing and intertemporal decision making in adults and adolescents: the role of impulsivity and decision consistency.
    Ripke S; Hübner T; Mennigen E; Müller KU; Rodehacke S; Schmidt D; Jacob MJ; Smolka MN
    Brain Res; 2012 Oct; 1478():36-47. PubMed ID: 22940231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. When is it adaptive to be patient? A general framework for evaluating delayed rewards.
    Fawcett TW; McNamara JM; Houston AI
    Behav Processes; 2012 Feb; 89(2):128-36. PubMed ID: 21920413
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Intertemporal choice in lemurs.
    Stevens JR; Mühlhoff N
    Behav Processes; 2012 Feb; 89(2):121-7. PubMed ID: 22024661
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The ecological rationality of delay tolerance: insights from capuchin monkeys.
    Addessi E; Paglieri F; Focaroli V
    Cognition; 2011 Apr; 119(1):142-7. PubMed ID: 21146163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Reward and decision processes in the brains of humans and nonhuman primates.
    Sirigu A; Duhamel JR
    Dialogues Clin Neurosci; 2016 Mar; 18(1):45-53. PubMed ID: 27069379
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The Web-Surf Task: A translational model of human decision-making.
    Abram SV; Breton YA; Schmidt B; Redish AD; MacDonald AW
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2016 Feb; 16(1):37-50. PubMed ID: 26377334
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The neuroscience of impulsive and self-controlled decisions.
    Kalenscher T; Ohmann T; Güntürkün O
    Int J Psychophysiol; 2006 Nov; 62(2):203-11. PubMed ID: 16828187
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Speed-accuracy tradeoffs in animal decision making.
    Chittka L; Skorupski P; Raine NE
    Trends Ecol Evol; 2009 Jul; 24(7):400-7. PubMed ID: 19409649
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Decision making and risk attitude of the common marmoset in a gambling task.
    Tokuno H; Tanaka I
    Neurosci Res; 2011 Nov; 71(3):260-5. PubMed ID: 21802456
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Decision making after traumatic brain injury: a temporal discounting paradigm.
    Wood RL; McHugh L
    J Int Neuropsychol Soc; 2013 Feb; 19(2):181-8. PubMed ID: 23298735
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Discounting of delayed rewards is not hyperbolic.
    Luhmann CC
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2013 Jul; 39(4):1274-9. PubMed ID: 23356243
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.