These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

82 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16406373)

  • 1. Timing, memory for intervals, and memory for untimed stimuli: The role of instructional ambiguity.
    Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):88-97. PubMed ID: 16406373
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Timing, memory for intervals, and memory for untimed stimuli: the role of instructional ambiguity.
    Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2005 Nov; 70(3):209-22. PubMed ID: 16095851
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Interval timing with gaps: gap ambiguity as an alternative to temporal decay.
    Zentall TR; Kaiser DH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Oct; 31(4):484-6. PubMed ID: 16248734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Timing in pigeons: effects of the similarity between intertrial interval and gap in a timing signal.
    Kaiser DH; Zentall TR; Neiman E
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2002 Oct; 28(4):416-22. PubMed ID: 12395499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Temporal discrimination learning by pigeons.
    Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2007 Feb; 74(2):286-92. PubMed ID: 17110057
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Time-sharing in pigeons: Independent effects of gap duration, position and discriminability from the timed signal.
    Buhusi CV; Paskalis JP; Cerutti DT
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):116-25. PubMed ID: 16414210
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Time sharing in rats: A peak-interval procedure with gaps and distracters.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):107-15. PubMed ID: 16413701
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Memory for timing visual and auditory signals in albino and pigmented rats.
    Buhusi CV; Perera D; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Jan; 31(1):18-30. PubMed ID: 15656724
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Support for a theory of memory for event duration must distinguish between test-trial ambiguity and actual memory loss.
    Zentall TR
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1999 Nov; 72(3):467-72. PubMed ID: 10605105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?
    Wearden JH; Parry A; Stamp L
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 2002 Jan; 55(1):1-25. PubMed ID: 11900304
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Performance of spontaneously hypertensive rats in a peak-interval procedure with gaps.
    Orduña V; García A; Menez M; Hong E; Bouzas A
    Behav Brain Res; 2008 Aug; 191(1):72-6. PubMed ID: 18436313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Temporal memory in mature and aged rats is sensitive to choline acetyltransferase inhibition.
    Meck WH
    Brain Res; 2006 Sep; 1108(1):168-75. PubMed ID: 16854393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Interval timing: memory, not a clock.
    Staddon JE
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2005 Jul; 9(7):312-4. PubMed ID: 15953755
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Interval timing by an invertebrate, the bumble bee Bombus impatiens.
    Boisvert MJ; Sherry DF
    Curr Biol; 2006 Aug; 16(16):1636-40. PubMed ID: 16920625
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: a comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing.
    McAuley JD; Jones MR
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Dec; 29(6):1102-25. PubMed ID: 14640833
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Interval timing with gaps and distracters: evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2006 Jul; 32(3):329-38. PubMed ID: 16834500
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Timing for the absence of a stimulus: the gap paradigm reversed.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2000 Jul; 26(3):305-22. PubMed ID: 10913995
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The perception of empty and filled time intervals by rats.
    Santi A; Miki A; Hornyak S; Eidse J
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):144-56. PubMed ID: 16406400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Daily meal timing is not necessary for resetting the main circadian clock by calorie restriction.
    Mendoza J; Drevet K; Pévet P; Challet E
    J Neuroendocrinol; 2008 Feb; 20(2):251-60. PubMed ID: 18088363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Timing of multiple overlapping intervals: how many clocks do we have?
    van Rijn H; Taatgen NA
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2008 Nov; 129(3):365-75. PubMed ID: 18849020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.