These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

125 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16615395)

  • 1. Mixing costs in task shifting reflect sequential processing stages in a multicomponent task.
    Steinhauser M; Hübner R
    Mem Cognit; 2005 Dec; 33(8):1484-94. PubMed ID: 16615395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions.
    Philipp AM; Kalinich C; Koch I; Schubotz RI
    Psychol Res; 2008 Jul; 72(4):405-14. PubMed ID: 18443820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Age differences in response selection for pure and mixed stimulus-response mappings and tasks.
    Vu KP; Proctor RW
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2008 Sep; 129(1):49-60. PubMed ID: 18541219
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Task selection cost asymmetry without task switching.
    Bryck RL; Mayr U
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2008 Feb; 15(1):128-34. PubMed ID: 18605492
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Total sleep deprivation increases the costs of shifting between simple cognitive tasks.
    Heuer H; Kleinsorge T; Klein W; Kohlisch O
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2004 Sep; 117(1):29-64. PubMed ID: 15288228
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Response execution, selection, or activation: what is sufficient for response-related repetition effects under task shifting?
    Hübner R; Druey MD
    Psychol Res; 2006 Jul; 70(4):245-61. PubMed ID: 16151720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Sequential dependencies in the Eriksen flanker task: a direct comparison of two competing accounts.
    Davelaar EJ; Stevens J
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2009 Feb; 16(1):121-6. PubMed ID: 19145021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Conflicts between expected and actually performed behavior lead to verbal report of incidentally acquired sequential knowledge.
    Haider H; Frensch PA
    Psychol Res; 2009 Nov; 73(6):817-34. PubMed ID: 19034498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The impact of stimulus-specific practice and task instructions on response congruency effects between tasks.
    Wendt M; Kiesel A
    Psychol Res; 2008 Jul; 72(4):425-32. PubMed ID: 17546462
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. On the representation of task information in task switching: evidence from task and dimension switching.
    Vandierendonck A; Christiaens E; Liefooghe B
    Mem Cognit; 2008 Oct; 36(7):1248-61. PubMed ID: 18927041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Task shifting in dual-task settings.
    Hsieh S
    Percept Mot Skills; 2002 Apr; 94(2):407-14. PubMed ID: 12027331
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. No age differences in complex memory search: older adults search as efficiently as younger adults.
    Lange EB; Verhaeghen P
    Psychol Aging; 2009 Mar; 24(1):105-15. PubMed ID: 19290742
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Assembling a task space: global determination of local shift costs.
    Kleinsorge T; Heuer H; Schmidtke V
    Psychol Res; 2004 Feb; 68(1):31-40. PubMed ID: 12774231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Voluntary task switching under load: contribution of top-down and bottom-up factors in goal-directed behavior.
    Demanet J; Verbruggen F; Liefooghe B; Vandierendonck A
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2010 Jun; 17(3):387-93. PubMed ID: 20551363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Implicit sequence learning is represented by stimulus-response rules.
    Schwarb H; Schumacher EH
    Mem Cognit; 2010 Sep; 38(6):677-88. PubMed ID: 20852232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Context dependent learning in the serial RT task.
    Abrahamse EL; Verwey WB
    Psychol Res; 2008 Jul; 72(4):397-404. PubMed ID: 17674034
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. More insight into the interplay of response selection and visual attention in dual-tasks: masked visual search and response selection are performed in parallel.
    Reimer CB; Schubert T
    Psychol Res; 2019 Apr; 83(3):459-475. PubMed ID: 28917014
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Unmixing the mixing cost: contributions from dimensional relevance and stimulus-response suppression.
    Marí-Beffa P; Cooper S; Houghton G
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2012 Apr; 38(2):478-88. PubMed ID: 22060145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Shifting the set of stimulus selection when switching between tasks.
    Wendt M; Luna-Rodriguez A; Jacobsen T
    Psychol Res; 2018 Jan; 82(1):134-145. PubMed ID: 28752317
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Hierarchical control of cognitive processes: switching tasks in sequences.
    Schneider DW; Logan GD
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2006 Nov; 135(4):623-40. PubMed ID: 17087577
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.