These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
236 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16683931)
1. Dual processing in reasoning: two systems but one reasoner. De Neys W Psychol Sci; 2006 May; 17(5):428-33. PubMed ID: 16683931 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Belief inhibition in children's reasoning: memory-based evidence. Steegen S; De Neys W J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Jun; 112(2):231-42. PubMed ID: 22402412 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Neural correlates of dual-task effect on belief-bias syllogistic reasoning: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Tsujii T; Watanabe S Brain Res; 2009 Sep; 1287():118-25. PubMed ID: 19577547 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Automatic-heuristic and executive-analytic processing during reasoning: Chronometric and dual-task considerations. De Neys W Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2006 Jun; 59(6):1070-100. PubMed ID: 16885144 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Representational and executive selection resources in 'theory of mind': evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. German TP; Hehman JA Cognition; 2006 Aug; 101(1):129-52. PubMed ID: 16288734 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Logic and belief across the lifespan: the rise and fall of belief inhibition during syllogistic reasoning. De Neys W; Van Gelder E Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):123-30. PubMed ID: 19120420 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Belief inhibition during thinking: not always winning but at least taking part. De Neys W; Franssens S Cognition; 2009 Oct; 113(1):45-61. PubMed ID: 19703685 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. De Neys W; Glumicic T Cognition; 2008 Mar; 106(3):1248-99. PubMed ID: 17631876 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. The logic-bias effect: The role of effortful processing in the resolution of belief-logic conflict. Howarth S; Handley SJ; Walsh C Mem Cognit; 2016 Feb; 44(2):330-49. PubMed ID: 26390872 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Working memory, metacognitive uncertainty, and belief bias in syllogistic reasoning. Quayle JD; Ball LJ Q J Exp Psychol A; 2000 Nov; 53(4):1202-23. PubMed ID: 11131820 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Highly reflective reasoners show no signs of belief inhibition. Svedholm-Häkkinen AM Acta Psychol (Amst); 2015 Jan; 154():69-76. PubMed ID: 25499057 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Evidence that logical reasoning depends on conscious processing. DeWall CN; Baumeister RF; Masicampo EJ Conscious Cogn; 2008 Sep; 17(3):628-45. PubMed ID: 18226923 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Emotion-based learning and central executive resources: an investigation of intuition and the Iowa Gambling Task. Turnbull OH; Evans CE; Bunce A; Carzolio B; O'connor J Brain Cogn; 2005 Apr; 57(3):244-7. PubMed ID: 15780457 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]