These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

109 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16719656)

  • 1. How to hit Scylla without avoiding Charybdis: comment on Perruchet, Tyler, Galland, and Peereman (2004).
    Bonatti LL; Peña M; Nespor M; Mehler J
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2006 May; 135(2):314-21; discussion 322-6. PubMed ID: 16719656
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Primitive computations in speech processing.
    Endress AD; Mehler J
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Nov; 62(11):2187-209. PubMed ID: 19418378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Rapid learning of syllable classes from a perceptually continuous speech stream.
    Endress AD; Bonatti LL
    Cognition; 2007 Nov; 105(2):247-99. PubMed ID: 17083927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The role of pause cues in language learning: the emergence of event-related potentials related to sequence processing.
    Mueller JL; Bahlmann J; Friederici AD
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2008 May; 20(5):892-905. PubMed ID: 18201121
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Learning nonadjacent dependencies: no need for algebraic-like computations.
    Perruchet P; Tyler MD; Galland N; Peereman R
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2004 Dec; 133(4):573-83. PubMed ID: 15584807
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Does grammar constrain statistical learning? Commentary on Bonatti, Peña, Nespor, and Mehler (2005).
    Keidel JL; Jenison RL; Kluender KR; Seidenberg MS
    Psychol Sci; 2007 Oct; 18(10):922-3. PubMed ID: 17894611
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Signal-driven computations in speech processing.
    Peña M; Bonatti LL; Nespor M; Mehler J
    Science; 2002 Oct; 298(5593):604-7. PubMed ID: 12202684
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Finding words and rules in a speech stream: functional differences between vowels and consonants.
    Toro JM; Nespor M; Mehler J; Bonatti LL
    Psychol Sci; 2008 Feb; 19(2):137-44. PubMed ID: 18271861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Words, rules, and mechanisms of language acquisition.
    Endress AD; Bonatti LL
    Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci; 2016; 7(1):19-35. PubMed ID: 26683248
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infant word segmentation revisited: edge alignment facilitates target extraction.
    Seidl A; Johnson EK
    Dev Sci; 2006 Nov; 9(6):565-73. PubMed ID: 17059453
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The logical syntax of number words: theory, acquisition and processing.
    Musolino J
    Cognition; 2009 Apr; 111(1):24-45. PubMed ID: 19217616
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Decisions, decisions: infant language learning when multiple generalizations are possible.
    Gerken L
    Cognition; 2006 Jan; 98(3):B67-74. PubMed ID: 15992791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Statistical computations over a speech stream in a rodent.
    Toro JM; Trobalón JB
    Percept Psychophys; 2005 Jul; 67(5):867-75. PubMed ID: 16334058
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The quest for generalizations over consonants: asymmetries between consonants and vowels are not the by-product of acoustic differences.
    Toro JM; Shukla M; Nespor M; Endress AD
    Percept Psychophys; 2008 Nov; 70(8):1515-25. PubMed ID: 19064494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Bootstrapping word order in prelexical infants: a Japanese-Italian cross-linguistic study.
    Gervain J; Nespor M; Mazuka R; Horie R; Mehler J
    Cogn Psychol; 2008 Aug; 57(1):56-74. PubMed ID: 18241850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Why is language unique to humans?
    Mehler J; Nespor M; Shukla M; Peña M
    Novartis Found Symp; 2006; 270():251-80; discussion 280-92. PubMed ID: 16649719
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French infants.
    Höhle B; Bijeljac-Babic R; Herold B; Weissenborn J; Nazzi T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Jun; 32(3):262-74. PubMed ID: 19427039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Trajectories emerging from discrete versus continuous processing models in phonological competitor tasks: a commentary on Spivey, Grosjean, and Knoblich (2005).
    van der Wel RP; Eder JR; Mitchel AD; Walsh MM; Rosenbaum DA
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2009 Apr; 35(2):588-94. PubMed ID: 19331511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. At 11 months, prosody still outranks statistics.
    Johnson EK; Seidl AH
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):131-41. PubMed ID: 19120421
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. A Bayesian framework for word segmentation: exploring the effects of context.
    Goldwater S; Griffiths TL; Johnson M
    Cognition; 2009 Jul; 112(1):21-54. PubMed ID: 19409539
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.