These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

160 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16724778)

  • 1. Cuing the dimension of a distractor: verbal cues of target identity also benefit same-dimension distractor singletons.
    Meeter M; Theeuwes J
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2006 Feb; 13(1):118-24. PubMed ID: 16724778
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Region-based shielding of visual search from salient distractors: Target detection is impaired with same- but not different-dimension distractors.
    Sauter M; Liesefeld HR; Zehetleitner M; Müller HJ
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2018 Apr; 80(3):622-642. PubMed ID: 29299850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Temporal attentional capture: effects of irrelevant singletons on rapid serial visual search.
    Dalton P; Lavie N
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2006 Oct; 13(5):881-5. PubMed ID: 17328389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings.
    Schreij D; Owens C; Theeuwes J
    Percept Psychophys; 2008 Feb; 70(2):208-18. PubMed ID: 18372744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Attentional selection of objects or features: evidence from a modified search task.
    Mounts JR; Melara RD
    Percept Psychophys; 1999 Feb; 61(2):322-41. PubMed ID: 10089764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Goal-driven modulation as a function of time in saccadic target selection.
    van Zoest W; Donk M
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2008 Oct; 61(10):1553-72. PubMed ID: 17926227
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Learning to suppress salient distractors in the target dimension: Region-based inhibition is persistent and transfers to distractors in a nontarget dimension.
    Sauter M; Liesefeld HR; Müller HJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2019 Nov; 45(11):2080-2097. PubMed ID: 30688477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Overt and covert object-based attention.
    McCarley JS; Kramer AF; Peterson MS
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2002 Dec; 9(4):751-8. PubMed ID: 12613679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. A feature-weighting account of priming in conjunction search.
    Becker SI; Horstmann G
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2009 Feb; 71(2):258-72. PubMed ID: 19304616
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Auditory, tactile, and multisensory cues facilitate search for dynamic visual stimuli.
    Ngo MK; Spence C
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 Aug; 72(6):1654-65. PubMed ID: 20675808
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Learned and cued distractor rejection for multiple features in visual search.
    Stilwell BT; Vecera SP
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2019 Feb; 81(2):359-376. PubMed ID: 30421366
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Working memory, perceptual priming, and the perception of hierarchical forms: opposite effects of priming and working memory without memory refreshing.
    Kim JI; Humphreys GW
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 Aug; 72(6):1533-55. PubMed ID: 20675799
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Benefits from attention depend on the target type in location-precued discrimination.
    Cheal ML; Lyon DR
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 1992 Dec; 81(3):243-67. PubMed ID: 1462787
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Priming of luminance-defined motion direction in visual search.
    Kristjánsson A; Bjarnason A; Hjaltason AB; Stefánsdóttir BG
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2009 Jul; 71(5):1027-41. PubMed ID: 19525535
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Exogenous cuing of distractor location facilitates location selection by inhibition of return.
    Chao HF
    Exp Psychol; 2009; 56(2):121-7. PubMed ID: 19261587
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Luminance-increment detection: capacity-limited or not?
    Müller HJ; Humphreys GW
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 1991 Feb; 17(1):107-24. PubMed ID: 1826306
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Visual search for singleton feature targets across dimensions: Stimulus- and expectancy-driven effects in dimensional weighting.
    Muller HJ; Reimann B; Krummenacher J
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Oct; 29(5):1021-35. PubMed ID: 14585020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Binding targets' responses to distractors' locations: distractor response bindings in a location-priming task.
    Frings C; Möller B
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 Nov; 72(8):2176-83. PubMed ID: 21097861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Feature-based attention is functionally distinct from relation-based attention: The double dissociation between color-based capture and color-relation-based capture of attention.
    Du F; Jiao J
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2016 Apr; 42(4):480-93. PubMed ID: 26523490
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Contingent attentional capture or delayed allocation of attention?
    Remington RW; Folk CL; McLean JP
    Percept Psychophys; 2001 Feb; 63(2):298-307. PubMed ID: 11281104
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.