These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

200 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16733700)

  • 21. Contributions of gaze-centered and object-centered coding in a double-step saccade task.
    de Brouwer AJ; Medendorp WP; Smeets JB
    J Vis; 2016 Nov; 16(14):12. PubMed ID: 27846340
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. The effect of Kanizsa's compression illusion on reflexive saccades.
    Knox PC
    Exp Brain Res; 2006 Nov; 175(4):764-8. PubMed ID: 17051374
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. The Müller-Lyer illusion seen by the brain: an event-related brain potentials study.
    Qiu J; Li H; Zhang Q; Liu Q; Zhang F
    Biol Psychol; 2008 Feb; 77(2):150-8. PubMed ID: 17996352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The role of action intentionality and effector in the subjective expansion of temporal duration after saccadic eye movements.
    Melcher D; Kumar D; Srinivasan N
    Sci Rep; 2020 Oct; 10(1):16922. PubMed ID: 33037289
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Illusory shifts in visual direction accompany adaptation of saccadic eye movements.
    Bahcall DO; Kowler E
    Nature; 1999 Aug; 400(6747):864-6. PubMed ID: 10476963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Examining the crossmodal consequences of viewing the Müller-Lyer illusion.
    Gallace A; Spence C
    Exp Brain Res; 2005 May; 162(4):490-6. PubMed ID: 15616811
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Grasping the Müller-Lyer illusion: not a change in perceived length.
    Biegstraaten M; de Grave DD; Brenner E; Smeets JB
    Exp Brain Res; 2007 Jan; 176(3):497-503. PubMed ID: 17146650
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Achievement of transsaccadic visual stability using presaccadic and postsaccadic visual information.
    Honda H
    Vision Res; 2006 Oct; 46(20):3483-93. PubMed ID: 16828140
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. The effect of line-figure information on the magnitude of the dot forms of the Poggendorff and Müller-Lyer illusions.
    Predebon J
    Perception; 1986; 15(4):483-90. PubMed ID: 3822734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Decrement of the Müller-Lyer and Poggendorff illusions: the effects of inspection and practice.
    Predebon J
    Psychol Res; 2006 Sep; 70(5):384-94. PubMed ID: 16059736
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Consistent chronostasis effects across saccade categories imply a subcortical efferent trigger.
    Yarrow K; Johnson H; Haggard P; Rothwell JC
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Jun; 16(5):839-47. PubMed ID: 15200711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Ventral and dorsal stream interactions during the perception of the Müller-Lyer illusion: evidence derived from fMRI and dynamic causal modeling.
    Plewan T; Weidner R; Eickhoff SB; Fink GR
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2012 Oct; 24(10):2015-29. PubMed ID: 22721374
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Saccadic eye movements and perceptual judgments reveal a shared visual representation that is increasingly accurate over time.
    van Zoest W; Hunt AR
    Vision Res; 2011 Jan; 51(1):111-9. PubMed ID: 20951719
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Manual-aiming bias and the Müller-Lyer illusion: the roles of position and extent information.
    Predebon J
    Exp Brain Res; 2005 May; 163(1):100-8. PubMed ID: 15654590
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Effect of practice versus information on the visual illusion.
    Khorasani AA; Fadardi JS; Fadardi MS; Cox WM; Sharif JT
    Neurosci Bull; 2007 Jan; 23(1):30-4. PubMed ID: 17592522
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Shift in saccadic direction induced in humans by proprioceptive manipulation: a comparison between memory-guided and visually guided saccades.
    Allin F; Velay JL; Bouquerel A
    Exp Brain Res; 1996 Aug; 110(3):473-81. PubMed ID: 8871106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Diamond-winged variants of the Müller-Lyer figure: a test of Virsu's (1971) centroid theory.
    DeLucia PR; Longmire SP; Kennish J
    Percept Psychophys; 1994 Mar; 55(3):287-95. PubMed ID: 8036110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. A test of the spatial-frequency explanation of the Müller-Lyer illusion.
    Carrasco M; Figueroa JG; Willen JD
    Perception; 1986; 15(5):553-62. PubMed ID: 3588215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Different spatial representations guide eye and hand movements.
    Lisi M; Cavanagh P
    J Vis; 2017 Feb; 17(2):12. PubMed ID: 28245493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The effect of response uncertainty on illusory biases of perception and action.
    Keetch KM; Glazebrook CM; Lyons J; Lam MY; Weeks DJ; Elliott D
    Neurosci Lett; 2006 Oct; 406(1-2):117-21. PubMed ID: 16901634
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.