BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

137 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16781700)

  • 1. Testing the domain-specificity of a theory of mind deficit in brain-injured patients: evidence for consistent performance on non-verbal, "reality-unknown" false belief and false photograph tasks.
    Apperly IA; Samson D; Chiavarino C; Bickerton WL; Humphreys GW
    Cognition; 2007 May; 103(2):300-21. PubMed ID: 16781700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands.
    Apperly IA; Samson D; Chiavarino C; Humphreys GW
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Dec; 16(10):1773-84. PubMed ID: 15701227
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Representing metarepresentations: is there theory of mind-specific cognition?
    Egeth M; Kurzban R
    Conscious Cogn; 2009 Mar; 18(1):244-54. PubMed ID: 18805706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Error analyses reveal contrasting deficits in "theory of mind": neuropsychological evidence from a 3-option false belief task.
    Samson D; Apperly IA; Humphreys GW
    Neuropsychologia; 2007 Jun; 45(11):2561-9. PubMed ID: 17451756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Theory of mind in Williams syndrome assessed using a nonverbal task.
    Porter MA; Coltheart M; Langdon R
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2008 May; 38(5):806-14. PubMed ID: 17874179
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The curious incident of the photo that was accused of being false: issues of domain specificity in development, autism, and brain imaging.
    Perner J; Leekam S
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2008 Jan; 61(1):76-89. PubMed ID: 18038340
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Exploring links between language and cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Complement sentences, false belief, and executive functioning.
    Durrleman S; Franck J
    J Commun Disord; 2015; 54():15-31. PubMed ID: 25637130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective.
    Samson D; Apperly IA; Kathirgamanathan U; Humphreys GW
    Brain; 2005 May; 128(Pt 5):1102-11. PubMed ID: 15774506
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. [Theory of mind in Asperger syndrome].
    Duverger H; Da Fonseca D; Bailly D; Deruelle C
    Encephale; 2007 Sep; 33(4 Pt 1):592-7. PubMed ID: 18033148
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Do children with autism who pass false belief tasks understand the mind as active interpreter?
    Luckett T; Powell SD; Messer DJ; Thornton ME; Schulz J
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2002 Apr; 32(2):127-40. PubMed ID: 12058841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Nonspecificity and theory of mind: new evidence from a nonverbal false-sign task and children with autism spectrum disorders.
    Iao LS; Leekam SR
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Jun; 122():1-20. PubMed ID: 24508666
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding.
    Phillips LH; Bull R; Allen R; Insch P; Burr K; Ogg W
    Cognition; 2011 Aug; 120(2):236-47. PubMed ID: 21624567
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. False-belief understanding in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease.
    Fernandez-Duque D; Baird JA; Black SE
    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol; 2009 May; 31(4):489-97. PubMed ID: 18686116
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. False belief and sentence complement performance in children with specific language impairment.
    Miller C
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2004; 39(2):191-213. PubMed ID: 15204451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. My belief or yours? Differential theory of mind deficits in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease.
    Le Bouc R; Lenfant P; Delbeuck X; Ravasi L; Lebert F; Semah F; Pasquier F
    Brain; 2012 Oct; 135(Pt 10):3026-38. PubMed ID: 23065791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Memory for/and execution of future intentions: evidence from patients with Herpes Simplex Encephalitis.
    Sgaramella TM; Borgo F; Fenzo F; Garofalo P; Toso V
    Brain Cogn; 2000; 43(1-3):388-92. PubMed ID: 10857732
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. False Belief vs. False Photographs: A Test of Theory of Mind or Working Memory?
    Callejas A; Shulman GL; Corbetta M
    Front Psychol; 2011; 2():316. PubMed ID: 22069395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Theory of mind difficulties in patients with alcohol dependence: beyond the prefrontal cortex dysfunction hypothesis.
    Maurage F; de Timary P; Tecco JM; Lechantre S; Samson D
    Alcohol Clin Exp Res; 2015 Jun; 39(6):980-8. PubMed ID: 26033203
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. What do executive factors contribute to the failure on false belief tasks by children with autism?
    Russell J; Saltmarsh R; Hill E
    J Child Psychol Psychiatry; 1999 Sep; 40(6):859-68. PubMed ID: 10509881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Deception dissociates from false belief reasoning in deaf children: implications for the implicit versus explicit theory of mind distinction.
    de Villiers PA; de Villiers JG
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):188-209. PubMed ID: 22429041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.