These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

126 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 16834500)

  • 1. Interval timing with gaps and distracters: evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2006 Jul; 32(3):329-38. PubMed ID: 16834500
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Time sharing in rats: A peak-interval procedure with gaps and distracters.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):107-15. PubMed ID: 16413701
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Interval timing with gaps: gap ambiguity as an alternative to temporal decay.
    Zentall TR; Kaiser DH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Oct; 31(4):484-6. PubMed ID: 16248734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Time-sharing in rats: effect of distracter intensity and discriminability.
    Buhusi CV
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Jan; 38(1):30-9. PubMed ID: 22122061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Time-sharing in pigeons: Independent effects of gap duration, position and discriminability from the timed signal.
    Buhusi CV; Paskalis JP; Cerutti DT
    Behav Processes; 2006 Feb; 71(2-3):116-25. PubMed ID: 16414210
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Timing in pigeons: effects of the similarity between intertrial interval and gap in a timing signal.
    Kaiser DH; Zentall TR; Neiman E
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2002 Oct; 28(4):416-22. PubMed ID: 12395499
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Timing, memory for intervals, and memory for untimed stimuli: the role of instructional ambiguity.
    Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2005 Nov; 70(3):209-22. PubMed ID: 16095851
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The effect of intruded events on peak time: the role of reinforcement history during the intruded event.
    Aum S; Brown BL; Hemmes NS
    Behav Processes; 2007 Feb; 74(2):187-97. PubMed ID: 17157998
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Memory for timing visual and auditory signals in albino and pigmented rats.
    Buhusi CV; Perera D; Meck WH
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Jan; 31(1):18-30. PubMed ID: 15656724
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Effect of distracter preexposure on the reset of an internal clock.
    Buhusi CV; Matthews AR
    Behav Processes; 2014 Jan; 101():72-80. PubMed ID: 24056240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Do rats time filled and empty intervals of equal duration differently?
    Macinnis ML
    Behav Processes; 2007 Jun; 75(2):182-7. PubMed ID: 17343993
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Contrasting effects of interference and of breaks in interval timing.
    Gaudreault R; Fortin C; Macar F
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2010 Jan; 133(1):3-16. PubMed ID: 19716109
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The effect of an intruded event on peak-interval timing in rats: isolation of a postcue effect.
    Brown BL; Richer P; Doyère V
    Behav Processes; 2007 Mar; 74(3):300-10. PubMed ID: 17161921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Time and memory: towards a pacemaker-free theory of interval timing.
    Staddon JE; Higa JJ
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1999 Mar; 71(2):215-51. PubMed ID: 10220931
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Sex differences in interval timing and attention to time in C57Bl/6J mice.
    Buhusi M; Bartlett MJ; Buhusi CV
    Behav Brain Res; 2017 May; 324():96-99. PubMed ID: 28212945
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Intertrial-interval effects on sensitivity (A') and response bias (B") in a temporal discrimination by rats.
    Raslear TG; Shurtleff D; Simmons L
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1992 Nov; 58(3):527-35. PubMed ID: 1447544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Relative time sharing: new findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing.
    Buhusi CV; Meck WH
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2009 Jul; 364(1525):1875-85. PubMed ID: 19487190
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. No evidence for qualitative differences in the processing of short and long temporal intervals.
    Rammsayer T; Ulrich R
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2005 Oct; 120(2):141-71. PubMed ID: 15907778
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Elaborative rehearsal of nontemporal information interferes with temporal processing of durations in the range of seconds but not milliseconds.
    Rammsayer T; Ulrich R
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2011 May; 137(1):127-33. PubMed ID: 21474111
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Investigations of timing during the schedule and reinforcement intervals with wheel-running reinforcement.
    Belke TW; Christie-Fougere MM
    Behav Processes; 2006 Nov; 73(3):240-7. PubMed ID: 16844324
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.