BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

193 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17136553)

  • 1. Traplining in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens): a foraging strategy's ontogeny and the importance of spatial reference memory in short-range foraging.
    Saleh N; Chittka L
    Oecologia; 2007 Apr; 151(4):719-30. PubMed ID: 17136553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Inter-individual variability in the foraging behaviour of traplining bumblebees.
    Klein S; Pasquaretta C; Barron AB; Devaud JM; Lihoreau M
    Sci Rep; 2017 Jul; 7(1):4561. PubMed ID: 28676725
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Travel optimization by foraging bumblebees through readjustments of traplines after discovery of new feeding locations.
    Lihoreau M; Chittka L; Raine NE
    Am Nat; 2010 Dec; 176(6):744-57. PubMed ID: 20973670
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Spatial encoding by bumblebees (Bombus impatiens) of a reward within an artificial flower array.
    Church DL; Plowright CM
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):131-40. PubMed ID: 16416106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Bees do not use nearest-neighbour rules for optimization of multi-location routes.
    Lihoreau M; Chittka L; Le Comber SC; Raine NE
    Biol Lett; 2012 Feb; 8(1):13-6. PubMed ID: 21849311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. A simple iterative model accurately captures complex trapline formation by bumblebees across spatial scales and flower arrangements.
    Reynolds AM; Lihoreau M; Chittka L
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2013; 9(3):e1002938. PubMed ID: 23505353
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Bumblebees learn foraging routes through exploitation-exploration cycles.
    Kembro JM; Lihoreau M; Garriga J; Raposo EP; Bartumeus F
    J R Soc Interface; 2019 Jul; 16(156):20190103. PubMed ID: 31288648
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. A model of resource partitioning between foraging bees based on learning.
    Dubois T; Pasquaretta C; Barron AB; Gautrais J; Lihoreau M
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2021 Jul; 17(7):e1009260. PubMed ID: 34319987
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Monitoring Flower Visitation Networks and Interactions between Pairs of Bumble Bees in a Large Outdoor Flight Cage.
    Lihoreau M; Chittka L; Raine NE
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(3):e0150844. PubMed ID: 26982030
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Trade-off between travel distance and prioritization of high-reward sites in traplining bumblebees.
    Lihoreau M; Chittka L; Raine NE; Kudo G
    Funct Ecol; 2011 Dec; 25(6):1284-1292. PubMed ID: 22267886
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Do inexperienced bumblebee foragers use scent marks as social information?
    Leadbeater E; Chittka L
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Nov; 14(6):915-9. PubMed ID: 21643814
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Radar tracking and motion-sensitive cameras on flowers reveal the development of pollinator multi-destination routes over large spatial scales.
    Lihoreau M; Raine NE; Reynolds AM; Stelzer RJ; Lim KS; Smith AD; Osborne JL; Chittka L
    PLoS Biol; 2012; 10(9):e1001392. PubMed ID: 23049479
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Continuous Radar Tracking Illustrates the Development of Multi-destination Routes of Bumblebees.
    Woodgate JL; Makinson JC; Lim KS; Reynolds AM; Chittka L
    Sci Rep; 2017 Dec; 7(1):17323. PubMed ID: 29230062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Evidence of trapline foraging in honeybees.
    Buatois A; Lihoreau M
    J Exp Biol; 2016 Aug; 219(Pt 16):2426-9. PubMed ID: 27307487
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Bumblebees can discriminate between scent-marks deposited by conspecifics.
    Pearce RF; Giuggioli L; Rands SA
    Sci Rep; 2017 Mar; 7():43872. PubMed ID: 28266572
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Pollen foraging: learning a complex motor skill by bumblebees (Bombus terrestris).
    Raine NE; Chittka L
    Naturwissenschaften; 2007 Jun; 94(6):459-64. PubMed ID: 17149583
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Multimodal cues provide redundant information for bumblebees when the stimulus is visually salient, but facilitate red target detection in a naturalistic background.
    Telles FJ; Corcobado G; Trillo A; Rodríguez-Gironés MA
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(9):e0184760. PubMed ID: 28898287
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Foraging bumblebees avoid flowers already visited by conspecifics or by other bumblebee species.
    Goulson D; Hawson SA; Stout JC
    Anim Behav; 1998 Jan; 55(1):199-206. PubMed ID: 9480686
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Foraging scent marks of bumblebees: footprint cues rather than pheromone signals.
    Wilms J; Eltz T
    Naturwissenschaften; 2008 Feb; 95(2):149-53. PubMed ID: 17724572
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Winter active bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) achieve high foraging rates in urban Britain.
    Stelzer RJ; Chittka L; Carlton M; Ings TC
    PLoS One; 2010 Mar; 5(3):e9559. PubMed ID: 20221445
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.