These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

103 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17484580)

  • 1. U-shaped development in math: 7-year-olds outperform 9-year-olds on equivalence problems.
    McNeil NM
    Dev Psychol; 2007 May; 43(3):687-95. PubMed ID: 17484580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Limitations to teaching children 2 + 2 = 4: typical arithmetic problems can hinder learning of mathematical equivalence.
    McNeil NM
    Child Dev; 2008; 79(5):1524-37. PubMed ID: 18826540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Working memory and literacy as predictors of performance on algebraic word problems.
    Lee K; Ng SF; Ng EL; Lim ZY
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2004 Oct; 89(2):140-58. PubMed ID: 15388303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Kindergartners' understanding of additive commutativity within the context of word problems.
    Wilkins JL; Baroody AJ; Tiilikainen S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 May; 79(1):23-36. PubMed ID: 11292309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Developing access to number magnitude: a study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds.
    van Galen MS; Reitsma P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2008 Oct; 101(2):99-113. PubMed ID: 18602112
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Number-word sequence skill and arithmetic performance.
    Johansson BS
    Scand J Psychol; 2005 Apr; 46(2):157-67. PubMed ID: 15762943
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Math in actions: actor mode reveals the true arithmetic abilities of French-speaking 2-year-olds in a magic task.
    Lubin A; Poirel N; Rossi S; Pineau A; Houdé O
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Jul; 103(3):376-85. PubMed ID: 19289239
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Individual differences in trajectories of arithmetical development in typically achieving 5- to 7-year-olds.
    Jordan JA; Mulhern G; Wylie J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Aug; 103(4):455-68. PubMed ID: 19296965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Improving postswitch performance in the dimensional change card-sorting task: the importance of the switch and of pretraining by redescribing the test cards.
    Mack W
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2007 Dec; 98(4):243-51. PubMed ID: 17599343
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Benefits of practicing 4 = 2 + 2: nontraditional problem formats facilitate children's understanding of mathematical equivalence.
    McNeil NM; Fyfe ER; Petersen LA; Dunwiddie AE; Brletic-Shipley H
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1620-33. PubMed ID: 21793817
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Children's intuitive mathematics: the development of knowledge about nonlinear growth.
    Ebersbach M; Wilkening F
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(1):296-308. PubMed ID: 17328706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. A longitudinal study of children's performance on simple multiplication and division problems.
    De Brauwer J; Fias W
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Sep; 45(5):1480-96. PubMed ID: 19702407
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The development of proof construction in middle childhood.
    Ricco RB
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1997 Sep; 66(3):279-310. PubMed ID: 9299077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Flankers facilitate 3-year-olds' performance in a card-sorting task.
    Jordan PL; Morton JB
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Jan; 44(1):265-74. PubMed ID: 18194025
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Visual spatial representation in mathematical problem solving by deaf and hearing students.
    Blatto-Vallee G; Kelly RR; Gaustad MG; Porter J; Fonzi J
    J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ; 2007; 12(4):432-48. PubMed ID: 17548803
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The trouble with transfer: insights from microgenetic changes in the representation of numerical magnitude.
    Opfer JE; Thompson CA
    Child Dev; 2008; 79(3):788-804. PubMed ID: 18489428
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement.
    De Smedt B; Verschaffel L; Ghesquière P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Aug; 103(4):469-79. PubMed ID: 19285682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Number words in young children's conceptual and procedural knowledge of addition, subtraction and inversion.
    Canobi KH; Bethune NE
    Cognition; 2008 Sep; 108(3):675-86. PubMed ID: 18621361
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The developmental relations between conceptual and procedural knowledge: a multimethod approach.
    Schneider M; Stern E
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Jan; 46(1):178-92. PubMed ID: 20053016
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Representational change and magnitude estimation: why young children can make more accurate salary comparisons than adults.
    Opfer JE; Devries JM
    Cognition; 2008 Sep; 108(3):843-9. PubMed ID: 18573491
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.