These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

180 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17676553)

  • 61. Cognitive aging and the adaptive use of recognition in decision making.
    Pachur T; Mata R; Schooler LJ
    Psychol Aging; 2009 Dec; 24(4):901-15. PubMed ID: 20025405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. The combined contributions of the cue-familiarity and accessibility heuristics to feelings of knowing.
    Koriat A; Levy-Sadot R
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2001 Jan; 27(1):34-53. PubMed ID: 11204106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. Are consistent juror decisions related to fast and frugal decision making? Investigating the relationship between juror consistency, decision speed and cue utilisation.
    Curley LJ; Murray J; MacLean R; Laybourn P
    Med Sci Law; 2017 Oct; 57(4):211-219. PubMed ID: 28992745
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. Long-term serial position effects in cue-based inference.
    Lawrence A; Thomas R; Dougherty M
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(7):e0200821. PubMed ID: 30021017
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. Response demands and the recruitment of heuristic strategies in syllogistic reasoning.
    Reverberi C; Rusconi P; Paulesu E; Cherubini P
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Mar; 62(3):513-30. PubMed ID: 18609389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Causal conjunction fallacies: the roles of causal strength and mental resources.
    Crisp AK; Feeney A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Dec; 62(12):2320-37. PubMed ID: 19391046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. On the suitability of fast and frugal heuristics for designing values clarification methods in patient decision aids: a critical analysis.
    Pieterse AH; de Vries M
    Health Expect; 2013 Sep; 16(3):e73-9. PubMed ID: 21902770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. From information processing to decisions: Formalizing and comparing psychologically plausible choice models.
    Heck DW; Hilbig BE; Moshagen M
    Cogn Psychol; 2017 Aug; 96():26-40. PubMed ID: 28601709
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. Automatic activation of attribute knowledge in heuristic inference from memory.
    Khader PH; Pachur T; Jost K
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2013 Apr; 20(2):372-7. PubMed ID: 23132606
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Satisficing in split-second decision making is characterized by strategic cue discounting.
    Oh H; Beck JM; Zhu P; Sommer MA; Ferrari S; Egner T
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2016 Dec; 42(12):1937-1956. PubMed ID: 27253846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. Take-the-best and the influence of decision-inconsistent attributes on decision confidence and choices in memory-based decisions.
    Dummel S; Rummel J
    Memory; 2016 Nov; 24(10):1435-43. PubMed ID: 26642913
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. Recognizing users of the recognition heuristic.
    Hilbig BE; Pohl RF
    Exp Psychol; 2008; 55(6):394-401. PubMed ID: 19130765
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Fast or frugal, but not both: Decision heuristics under time pressure.
    Bobadilla-Suarez S; Love BC
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Jan; 44(1):24-33. PubMed ID: 28557503
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. Very clever homunculus: compound stimulus strategies for the explicit task-cuing procedure.
    Logan GD; Bundesen C
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2004 Oct; 11(5):832-40. PubMed ID: 15732691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Evidence accumulation in decision making: unifying the "take the best" and the "rational" models.
    Lee MD; Cummins TD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2004 Apr; 11(2):343-52. PubMed ID: 15260204
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. Cognitive niches: an ecological model of strategy selection.
    Marewski JN; Schooler LJ
    Psychol Rev; 2011 Jul; 118(3):393-437. PubMed ID: 21744978
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. The limited use of the fluency heuristic: Converging evidence across different procedures.
    Pohl RF; Erdfelder E; Michalkiewicz M; Castela M; Hilbig BE
    Mem Cognit; 2016 Oct; 44(7):1114-26. PubMed ID: 27383467
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Inferential dependencies in causal inference: a comparison of belief-distribution and associative approaches.
    Carroll CD; Cheng PW; Lu H
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2013 Aug; 142(3):845-63. PubMed ID: 22963188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. Reconsidering "evidence" for fast-and-frugal heuristics.
    Hilbig BE
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2010 Dec; 17(6):923-30. PubMed ID: 21169591
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Do people treat missing information adaptively when making inferences?
    Garcia-Retamero R; Rieskamp J
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Oct; 62(10):1991-2013. PubMed ID: 19241222
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.