These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

120 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17685790)

  • 1. Both parallelism and orthogonality are used to perceive 3D slant of rectangles from 2D images.
    Saunders JA; Backus BT
    J Vis; 2007 Apr; 7(6):7. PubMed ID: 17685790
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The accuracy and reliability of perceived depth from linear perspective as a function of image size.
    Saunders JA; Backus BT
    J Vis; 2006 Aug; 6(9):933-54. PubMed ID: 17083286
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Perception of surface slant from oriented textures.
    Saunders JA; Backus BT
    J Vis; 2006 Aug; 6(9):882-97. PubMed ID: 17083282
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The role of the foreshortening cue in the perception of 3D object slant.
    Ivanov IV; Kramer DJ; Mullen KT
    Vision Res; 2014 Jan; 94():41-50. PubMed ID: 24216007
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant.
    Knill DC
    J Vis; 2007 May; 7(7):5.1-24. PubMed ID: 17685801
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Orientation-specific learning of the prior assumption for 3D slant perception.
    Taya S; Sato M
    Sci Rep; 2018 Jul; 8(1):11063. PubMed ID: 30038249
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. A laminar cortical model for 3D perception of slanted and curved surfaces and of 2D images: development, attention, and bistability.
    Grossberg S; Swaminathan G
    Vision Res; 2004 May; 44(11):1147-87. PubMed ID: 15050817
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Cue conflict and stereoscopic surface slant about horizontal and vertical axes.
    Ryan C; Gillam B
    Perception; 1994; 23(6):645-58. PubMed ID: 7845758
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Multiple texture cues are integrated for perception of 3D slant from texture.
    Chen Z; Saunders JA
    J Vis; 2020 Jul; 20(7):14. PubMed ID: 32678877
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Perception of 3D surface orientation from skew symmetry.
    Saunders JA; Knill DC
    Vision Res; 2001 Nov; 41(24):3163-83. PubMed ID: 11711141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Dissociations between slant-contrast and reversed slant-contrast.
    Poom L; Olsson H; Börjesson E
    Vision Res; 2007 Mar; 47(6):746-54. PubMed ID: 17300825
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Reliability-dependent contributions of visual orientation cues in parietal cortex.
    Rosenberg A; Angelaki DE
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2014 Dec; 111(50):18043-8. PubMed ID: 25427796
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The contributions of surface features and contour shapes to object slant perception.
    Niimi R
    Iperception; 2023; 14(2):20416695231160402. PubMed ID: 36923004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The framing effect with rectangular and trapezoidal surfaces: actual and pictorial surface slant, frame orientation, and viewing condition.
    Reinhardt-Rutland AH
    Perception; 1999; 28(11):1361-71. PubMed ID: 10755145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The perceived motion of a stereokinetic stimulus.
    Rokers B; Yuille A; Liu Z
    Vision Res; 2006 Jul; 46(15):2375-87. PubMed ID: 16540141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Slant cue are combined early in visual processing: evidence from visual search.
    Sousa R; Brenner E; Smeets JB
    Vision Res; 2009 Jan; 49(2):257-61. PubMed ID: 19027776
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Adapting internal statistical models for interpreting visual cues to depth.
    Seydell A; Knill DC; Trommershäuser J
    J Vis; 2010 Apr; 10(4):1.1-27. PubMed ID: 20465321
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Pictorial cues constrain depth in da Vinci stereopsis.
    Makino Y; Yano M
    Vision Res; 2006 Jan; 46(1-2):91-105. PubMed ID: 16271743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Amodal completion with background determines depth from monocular gap stereopsis.
    Grove PM; Ben Sachtler WL; Gillam BJ
    Vision Res; 2006 Oct; 46(22):3771-4. PubMed ID: 16938332
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Is pictorial perception robust? The effect of the observer vantage point on the perceived depth structure of linear-perspective images.
    Todorović D
    Perception; 2008; 37(1):106-25. PubMed ID: 18399251
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.