These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
43. Symbolic gesture versus word: is there a modality advantage for onset of symbol use? Goodwyn SW; Acredolo LP Child Dev; 1993 Jun; 64(3):688-701. PubMed ID: 8339689 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
44. Monolingual, bilingual, trilingual: infants' language experience influences the development of a word-learning heuristic. Byers-Heinlein K; Werker JF Dev Sci; 2009 Sep; 12(5):815-23. PubMed ID: 19702772 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
45. The role of comparison in preschoolers' novel object categorization. Graham SA; Namy LL; Gentner D; Meagher K J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):280-90. PubMed ID: 20643266 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
46. Location, location, location: development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy. Kirkham NZ; Slemmer JA; Richardson DC; Johnson SP Child Dev; 2007; 78(5):1559-71. PubMed ID: 17883448 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
47. Learning of syllable-object relations by preverbal infants: the role of temporal synchrony and syllable distinctiveness. Gogate LJ J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Mar; 105(3):178-97. PubMed ID: 20004909 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
48. What's in a shape? Children represent shape variability differently than adults when naming objects. Abecassis M; Sera MD; Yonas A; Schwade J J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Mar; 78(3):213-39. PubMed ID: 11222000 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
50. The role of gesture in children's learning to count. Graham TA J Exp Child Psychol; 1999 Dec; 74(4):333-55. PubMed ID: 10552922 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
51. On the relation between the acquisition of singular-plural morpho-syntax and the conceptual distinction between one and more than one. Barner D; Thalwitz D; Wood J; Yang SJ; Carey S Dev Sci; 2007 May; 10(3):365-73. PubMed ID: 17444976 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
52. Developmental differences in children's context-dependent word learning. Vlach HA; Sandhofer CM J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Feb; 108(2):394-401. PubMed ID: 21074169 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
53. Infants' perception of natural and distorted arrangements of a schematic face. Maurer D; Barrera M Child Dev; 1981 Mar; 52(1):196-202. PubMed ID: 7238143 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
54. Young infants encode lexical stress in newly encountered words. Curtin S J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Apr; 105(4):376-85. PubMed ID: 20089259 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
55. Infants use shared experience to interpret pointing gestures. Liebal K; Behne T; Carpenter M; Tomasello M Dev Sci; 2009 Mar; 12(2):264-71. PubMed ID: 19143799 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
56. Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants. Gredebäck G; Stasiewicz D; Falck-Ytter T; von Hofsten C; Rosander K Dev Psychol; 2009 Jul; 45(4):1190-4. PubMed ID: 19586188 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
57. Appreciating language conventions: thirteen-month-old Chinese infants understand that word generalization is shared practice. Liu S; Sun R J Child Lang; 2019 Jul; 46(4):812-823. PubMed ID: 31018878 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
58. Sequence learning in 4-month-old infants: do infants represent ordinal information? Lewkowicz DJ; Berent I Child Dev; 2009; 80(6):1811-23. PubMed ID: 19930353 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
59. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. Gelman SA; Markman EM Child Dev; 1987 Dec; 58(6):1532-41. PubMed ID: 3691200 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]