These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
227 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17723046)
1. Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Pasquini ES; Corriveau KH; Koenig M; Harris PL Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1216-26. PubMed ID: 17723046 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Preschoolers trust particular informants when learning new names and new morphological forms. Corriveau KH; Pickard K; Harris PL Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Mar; 29(Pt 1):46-63. PubMed ID: 21199505 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Choosing your informant: weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Corriveau K; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2009 Apr; 12(3):426-37. PubMed ID: 19371367 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Children monitor individuals' expertise for word learning. Sobel DM; Corriveau KH Child Dev; 2010; 81(2):669-79. PubMed ID: 20438467 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Five-year-olds are willing, but 4-year-olds refuse, to trust informants who offer new and unfamiliar labels for parts of the body. Luu B; Rosnay Md; Harris PL J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):234-46. PubMed ID: 23872524 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Children assess informant reliability using bystanders' non-verbal cues. Fusaro M; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):771-7. PubMed ID: 18801133 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurate informant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. Corriveau K; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):188-93. PubMed ID: 19120427 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. When being right is not enough: four-year-olds distinguish knowledgeable informants from merely accurate informants. Einav S; Robinson EJ Psychol Sci; 2011 Oct; 22(10):1250-3. PubMed ID: 21881060 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Characterizing children's expectations about expertise and incompetence: halo or pitchfork effects? Koenig MA; Jaswal VK Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1634-47. PubMed ID: 21790541 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Children's sensitivity to the conventionality of sources. Diesendruck G; Carmel N; Markson L Child Dev; 2010; 81(2):652-68. PubMed ID: 20438466 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Going with the flow: preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Corriveau KH; Fusaro M; Harris PL Psychol Sci; 2009 Mar; 20(3):372-7. PubMed ID: 19207691 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. The good, the strong, and the accurate: preschoolers' evaluations of informant attributes. Fusaro M; Corriveau KH; Harris PL J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Dec; 110(4):561-74. PubMed ID: 21802693 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Accuracy trumps accent in children's endorsement of object labels. Corriveau KH; Kinzler KD; Harris PL Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):470-9. PubMed ID: 23231692 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Matching and naming objects by shape or function: age and context effects in preschool children. Deák GO; Ray SD; Pick AD Dev Psychol; 2002 Jul; 38(4):503-18. PubMed ID: 12090481 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Do children trust based on group membership or prior accuracy? The role of novel group membership in children's trust decisions. Elashi FB; Mills CM J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Dec; 128():88-104. PubMed ID: 25108696 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Knowledge, concepts, and inferences in childhood. Farrar MJ; Raney GE; Boyer ME Child Dev; 1992 Jun; 63(3):673-91. PubMed ID: 1600830 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. In the absence of conflicting testimony young children trust inaccurate informants. Vanderbilt KE; Heyman GD; Liu D Dev Sci; 2014 May; 17(3):443-51. PubMed ID: 24444426 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Krogh-Jespersen S; Echols CH Child Dev; 2012; 83(2):581-90. PubMed ID: 22239543 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]