These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

150 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 17960729)

  • 21. Behavioral thermoregulation of wild Japanese macaques: comparisons between two subpopulations.
    Hanya G; Kiyono M; Hayaishi S
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Jul; 69(7):802-15. PubMed ID: 17294433
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Stone affordances as potential for action expression in object play in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Cenni C; Pellis SM; Wandia IN; Leca JB
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 Aug; 135(3):430-438. PubMed ID: 34553980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Wild capuchin monkeys spontaneously adjust actions when using hammer stones of different mass to crack nuts of different resistance.
    Liu Q; Fragaszy DM; Visalberghi E
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2016 Sep; 161(1):53-61. PubMed ID: 27135182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Comparisons of food availability and group density of Japanese macaques in primary, naturally regenerated, and plantation forests.
    Hanya G; Zamma K; Hayaishi S; Yoshihiro S; Tsuriya Y; Sugaya S; Kanaoka MM; Hayakawa S; Takahata Y
    Am J Primatol; 2005 Jul; 66(3):245-62. PubMed ID: 16015658
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Sex differences in the stone tool-use behavior of a wild population of burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea).
    Gumert MD; Hoong LK; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Primatol; 2011 Dec; 73(12):1239-49. PubMed ID: 21905061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Improving the welfare of captive macaques (Macaca sp.) through the use of water as enrichment.
    Robins JG; Waitt CD
    J Appl Anim Welf Sci; 2011; 14(1):75-84. PubMed ID: 21191849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Extra-large cluster formation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) on Shodoshima Island, central Japan, and related factors.
    Zhang P; Watanabe K
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Oct; 69(10):1119-30. PubMed ID: 17330869
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Mechanisms of expression of object play: A comparative study of stone handling in two captive groups of long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis fascicularis).
    Albanese V; Berardi R; Llorente M; Huffman MA; Leca JB
    Behav Processes; 2022 Nov; 203():104774. PubMed ID: 36328275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Habitual hot-spring bathing by a group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) in their natural habitat.
    Zhang P; Watanabe K; Eishi T
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Dec; 69(12):1425-30. PubMed ID: 17554750
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Topography-specific seed dispersal by Japanese macaques in a lowland forest on Yakushima Island, Japan.
    Tsujino R; Yumoto T
    J Anim Ecol; 2009 Jan; 78(1):119-25. PubMed ID: 19120599
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Age class differences in the feeding behavior of captive Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscataia) in the forested and nonvegetated enclosure groups.
    Jaman MF; Huffman MA
    Zoo Biol; 2011; 30(3):260-74. PubMed ID: 21656847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) in intertidal habitats.
    Gumert MD; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2012 Nov; 149(3):447-57. PubMed ID: 23042618
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Inferring functional patterns of tool use behavior from the temporal structure of object play sequences in a non-human primate species.
    Cenni C; Casarrubea M; Gunst N; Vasey PL; Pellis SM; Wandia IN; Leca JB
    Physiol Behav; 2020 Aug; 222():112938. PubMed ID: 32417233
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Matrilineal distribution of louse egg-handling techniques during grooming in free-ranging Japanese macaques.
    Tanaka I
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 1995 Oct; 98(2):197-201. PubMed ID: 8644879
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. From play to proficiency: The ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective.
    Tan AWY
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 May; 131(2):89-114. PubMed ID: 28318292
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Greater tool use diversity is associated with increased terrestriality in wild capuchin monkeys.
    Falótico T; Ottoni EB
    Am J Biol Anthropol; 2023 Jun; 181(2):312-317. PubMed ID: 37067352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Selection of effective stone tools by wild bearded capuchin monkeys.
    Visalberghi E; Addessi E; Truppa V; Spagnoletti N; Ottoni E; Izar P; Fragaszy D
    Curr Biol; 2009 Feb; 19(3):213-7. PubMed ID: 19147358
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Ranging patterns and habitat use of Sulawesi Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana) in a human-modified habitat.
    Riley EP
    Am J Primatol; 2008 Jul; 70(7):670-9. PubMed ID: 18454452
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Group-specific archaeological signatures of stone tool use in wild macaques.
    Luncz LV; Gill M; Proffitt T; Svensson MS; Kulik L; Malaivijitnond S
    Elife; 2019 Oct; 8():. PubMed ID: 31635691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Disability, compensatory behavior, and innovation in free-ranging adult female Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata).
    Turner SE; Fedigan LM; Matthews HD; Nakamichi M
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Sep; 74(9):788-803. PubMed ID: 22549480
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.