These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
4. Is this a dax which I see before me? Use of the logical argument disjunctive syllogism supports word-learning in children and adults. Halberda J Cogn Psychol; 2006 Dec; 53(4):310-44. PubMed ID: 16875685 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Learning words: children disregard some pragmatic information that conflicts with mutual exclusivity. Jaswal VK; Hansen MB Dev Sci; 2006 Mar; 9(2):158-65. PubMed ID: 16472316 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Preschoolers' extension of novel words to animals and artifacts. Graham SA; Welder AN; Merrifield BA; Berman JM J Child Lang; 2010 Sep; 37(4):913-27. PubMed ID: 19874639 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Rapid word learning in 13- and 17-month-olds in a naturalistic two-word procedure: looking versus reaching measures. Gurteen PM; Horne PJ; Erjavec M J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Jun; 109(2):201-17. PubMed ID: 21216414 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Preschoolers' and adults' reliance on object shape and object function for lexical extension. Graham SA; Williams LD; Huber JF J Exp Child Psychol; 1999 Oct; 74(2):128-51. PubMed ID: 10479398 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Specifying the relation between novel and known: input affects the acquisition of novel color terms. Gottfried GM; Tonks SJ Child Dev; 1996 Jun; 67(3):850-66. PubMed ID: 8706530 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Children's use of syntactic and pragmatic knowledge in the interpretation of novel adjectives. Diesendruck G; Hall DG; Graham SA Child Dev; 2006; 77(1):16-30. PubMed ID: 16460522 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Object movement in preschool children's word learning. Scofield J; Miller A; Hartin T J Child Lang; 2011 Jan; 38(1):181-200. PubMed ID: 20003577 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Do children with autism use the speaker's direction of gaze strategy to crack the code of language? Baron-Cohen S; Baldwin DA; Crowson M Child Dev; 1997 Feb; 68(1):48-57. PubMed ID: 9084124 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Preschoolers' appreciation of speaker vocal affect as a cue to referential intent. Berman JM; Chambers CG; Graham SA J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Oct; 107(2):87-99. PubMed ID: 20553796 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. How children block learning from ignorant speakers. Sabbagh MA; Shafman D Cognition; 2009 Sep; 112(3):415-22. PubMed ID: 19589508 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The role of inferences about referential intent in word learning: evidence from autism. Preissler MA; Carey S Cognition; 2005 Aug; 97(1):B13-23. PubMed ID: 15925356 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Two-year-olds exclude novel objects as potential referents of novel words based on pragmatics. Grassmann S; Stracke M; Tomasello M Cognition; 2009 Sep; 112(3):488-93. PubMed ID: 19616205 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Learning in context: linguistic and attentional constraints on children's color term learning. O'Hanlon CG; Roberson D J Exp Child Psychol; 2006 Aug; 94(4):275-300. PubMed ID: 16438979 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Young children's disambiguation of object name reference. Merriman WE; Schuster JM Child Dev; 1991 Dec; 62(6):1288-301. PubMed ID: 1786716 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]