These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
317 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 18395767)
1. Intertrial target-feature changes do not lead to more distraction by singletons: target uncertainty does. Lamy D; Yashar A Vision Res; 2008 May; 48(10):1274-9. PubMed ID: 18395767 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Priming of Pop-out provides reliable measures of target activation and distractor inhibition in selective attention. Lamy D; Antebi C; Aviani N; Carmel T Vision Res; 2008 Jan; 48(1):30-41. PubMed ID: 18054983 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. A search order lost effect: ignoring a singleton distractor affects visual search efficiency. Kumada T Vision Res; 2010 Jun; 50(14):1402-13. PubMed ID: 20025896 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Irrelevant singletons in pop-out search: attentional capture or filtering costs? Becker SI J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2007 Aug; 33(4):764-87. PubMed ID: 17683227 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Can intertrial priming account for the similarity effect in visual search? Becker SI; Ansorge U; Horstmann G Vision Res; 2009 Jul; 49(14):1738-56. PubMed ID: 19358862 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Target uncertainty does not lead to greater singleton distractor interference when target shapes are not interchangeable with nontarget shapes. Berry JH Vision Res; 2013 Mar; 80():31-40. PubMed ID: 23385060 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Lamy D; Egeth HE J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Oct; 29(5):1003-20. PubMed ID: 14585019 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Oculomotor capture by colour singletons depends on intertrial priming. Becker SI Vision Res; 2010 Oct; 50(21):2116-26. PubMed ID: 20691720 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. The stage of priming: are intertrial repetition effects attentional or decisional? Becker SI Vision Res; 2008 Feb; 48(5):664-84. PubMed ID: 18226831 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Irrelevant singletons in visual search do not capture attention but can produce nonspatial filtering costs. Wykowska A; Schubö A J Cogn Neurosci; 2011 Mar; 23(3):645-60. PubMed ID: 19929330 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Electrophysiological evidence of the capture of visual attention. Hickey C; McDonald JJ; Theeuwes J J Cogn Neurosci; 2006 Apr; 18(4):604-13. PubMed ID: 16768363 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Can intertrial effects of features and dimensions be explained by a single theory? Becker SI J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2008 Dec; 34(6):1417-40. PubMed ID: 19045983 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Electrophysiological indices of target and distractor processing in visual search. Hickey C; Di Lollo V; McDonald JJ J Cogn Neurosci; 2009 Apr; 21(4):760-75. PubMed ID: 18564048 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The role of target-distractor relationships in guiding attention and the eyes in visual search. Becker SI J Exp Psychol Gen; 2010 May; 139(2):247-65. PubMed ID: 20438251 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Intertrial repetition facilitates selection in time: common mechanisms underlie spatial and temporal search. Yashar A; Lamy D Psychol Sci; 2010 Feb; 21(2):243-51. PubMed ID: 20424053 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Absence of attentional capture in parallel search is possible: a failure to replicate attentional capture in a non-singleton target search task. Wienrich C; Janczyk M Atten Percept Psychophys; 2011 Oct; 73(7):2044-52. PubMed ID: 21805210 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Attentional capture by a salient distractor in visual search: the effect of target-distractor similarity. Poiese P; Spalek TM; Di Lollo V Can J Exp Psychol; 2008 Dec; 62(4):233-6. PubMed ID: 19071990 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]