These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
44. Numerical magnitude modulates temporal comparison: an ERP study. Xuan B; Chen XC; He S; Zhang DR Brain Res; 2009 May; 1269():135-42. PubMed ID: 19306851 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
45. Defining filled and empty space: reassessing the filled space illusion for active touch and vision. Collier ES; Lawson R Exp Brain Res; 2016 Sep; 234(9):2697-708. PubMed ID: 27233286 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
46. Modality differences in timing and the filled-duration illusion: Testing the pacemaker rate explanation. Williams EA; Yüksel EM; Stewart AJ; Jones LA Atten Percept Psychophys; 2019 Apr; 81(3):823-845. PubMed ID: 30569434 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
47. The role of keypecking during filled intervals on the judgment of time for empty and filled intervals by pigeons. Santi A; Adams A; Bassett J Learn Behav; 2010 Feb; 38(1):42-9. PubMed ID: 20065348 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
48. Cognitive abilities required in time judgment depending on the temporal tasks used: A comparison of children and adults. Droit-Volet S; Wearden JH; Zélanti PS Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2015; 68(11):2216-42. PubMed ID: 25730557 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
49. Timescale dependence in a conditional temporal discrimination procedure. Ludvig EA; Balci F; Longpre KM Behav Processes; 2008 Mar; 77(3):357-63. PubMed ID: 18006247 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
50. Differences in modal distortion in time perception due to working memory capacity: a response with a developmental study in children and adults. Droit-Volet S; Hallez Q Psychol Res; 2019 Oct; 83(7):1496-1505. PubMed ID: 29663130 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
51. Comparison of Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) and the Learning-to-Time (LeT) model in a successive temporal bisection task. Arantes J Behav Processes; 2008 Jun; 78(2):269-78. PubMed ID: 18280059 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
52. The Ebbinghaus illusion deceives adults but not young children. Doherty MJ; Campbell NM; Tsuji H; Phillips WA Dev Sci; 2010 Sep; 13(5):714-21. PubMed ID: 20712737 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
53. Alerting attention and time perception in children. Droit-Volet S J Exp Child Psychol; 2003 Aug; 85(4):372-84. PubMed ID: 12906848 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
55. The effect of filled and empty intervals on clock and memory processes in pigeons. Price E; Santi A Learn Behav; 2014 Jun; 42(2):153-63. PubMed ID: 24519455 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
57. Sleep duration and blood pressure in children: a cross-sectional study. Bayer O; Neuhauser H; von Kries R J Hypertens; 2009 Sep; 27(9):1789-93. PubMed ID: 19633568 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
58. One second interval production task during post-rotatory sensation. Capelli A; Israël I J Vestib Res; 2007; 17(5-6):239-49. PubMed ID: 18626135 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
59. A model of empty duration perception. Nakajima Y Perception; 1987; 16(4):485-520. PubMed ID: 3444731 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
60. The dynamic effect of context on interval timing in children and adults. Hallez Q; Damsma A; Rhodes D; van Rijn H; Droit-Volet S Acta Psychol (Amst); 2019 Jan; 192():87-93. PubMed ID: 30458315 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Previous] [Next] [New Search]