These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

160 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 18808253)

  • 21. Impaired action self-monitoring and cognitive confidence among ultra-high risk for psychosis and first-episode psychosis patients.
    Gawęda Ł; Li E; Lavoie S; Whitford TJ; Moritz S; Nelson B
    Eur Psychiatry; 2018 Jan; 47():67-75. PubMed ID: 29107832
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. False action memories in older adults: relationship with executive functions?
    Lindner I; Davidson PS
    Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn; 2014; 21(5):560-76. PubMed ID: 24128163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. The effects of emotional content on reality-monitoring performance in young and older adults.
    Kensinger EA; O'Brien JL; Swanberg K; Garoff-Eaton RJ; Schacter DL
    Psychol Aging; 2007 Dec; 22(4):752-64. PubMed ID: 18179295
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Self-generation and positivity effects following transcranial random noise stimulation in medial prefrontal cortex: A reality monitoring task in older adults.
    Mammarella N; Di Domenico A; Palumbo R; Fairfield B
    Cortex; 2017 Jun; 91():186-196. PubMed ID: 27912894
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Memory source confusions: effects of character rotation and sensory modality.
    Kahan TL
    Am J Psychol; 1996; 109(3):431-49. PubMed ID: 8837409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. High-confidence memory errors in old age: the roles of monitoring and binding processes.
    Fandakova Y; Shing YL; Lindenberger U
    Memory; 2013; 21(6):732-50. PubMed ID: 23305088
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Reality monitoring in recovering alcoholics.
    Burnett LJ; Marshall PH; Bell RW
    J Stud Alcohol; 1996 Mar; 57(2):210-6. PubMed ID: 8683971
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Episodic simulation of past and future events in older adults: Evidence from an experimental recombination task.
    Addis DR; Musicaro R; Pan L; Schacter DL
    Psychol Aging; 2010 Jun; 25(2):369-76. PubMed ID: 20545421
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Aging and qualitative characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined complex events.
    Hashtroudi S; Johnson MK; Chrosniak LD
    Psychol Aging; 1990 Mar; 5(1):119-26. PubMed ID: 2317290
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. The thin line between reality and imagination: The imagination inflation effect in Korsakoff's syndrome.
    El Haj M; Robin F
    Neuropsychology; 2020 Jul; 34(5):569-577. PubMed ID: 32281810
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. The Interaction Between Memory Trace and Memory Judgment in Age-Related Decline.
    Guillaume F; Tison C; Marzouki Y
    Exp Aging Res; 2015; 41(5):510-33. PubMed ID: 26524234
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Reality monitoring in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
    McNally RJ; Kohlbeck PA
    Behav Res Ther; 1993 Mar; 31(3):249-53. PubMed ID: 8476399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Perceptual reality monitoring: Neural mechanisms dissociating imagination from reality.
    Dijkstra N; Kok P; Fleming SM
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2022 Apr; 135():104557. PubMed ID: 35122782
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Adaptive constructive processes and memory accuracy: consequences of counterfactual simulations in young and older adults.
    Gerlach KD; Dornblaser DW; Schacter DL
    Memory; 2014; 22(1):145-62. PubMed ID: 23560477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Lateral posterior parietal activity during source memory judgments of perceived and imagined events.
    King DR; Miller MB
    Neuropsychologia; 2014 Jan; 53():122-36. PubMed ID: 24269856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Representational explanations of "process" dissociations in recognition: the DRYAD theory of aging and memory judgments.
    Benjamin AS
    Psychol Rev; 2010 Oct; 117(4):1055-79. PubMed ID: 20822289
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Source misattributions may increase the accuracy of source judgments.
    Lyle KB; Johnson MK
    Mem Cognit; 2007 Jul; 35(5):1024-33. PubMed ID: 17910186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Cross-modal source monitoring confusions between perceived and imagined events.
    Henkel LA; Franklin N; Johnson MK
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2000 Mar; 26(2):321-35. PubMed ID: 10764099
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Counterfactual imagination impairs memory for true actions: EEG and behavioural evidence.
    Dhammapeera P; Brunskill C; Hellerstedt R; Bergström ZM
    Cogn Neurosci; 2024 Jan; 15(1):12-23. PubMed ID: 38362597
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Destination memory accuracy and confidence in younger and older adults.
    Johnson TL; Jefferson SC
    Exp Aging Res; 2018; 44(1):62-81. PubMed ID: 29236576
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.