These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

190 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 18849020)

  • 41. Stop-reaction time and the internal clock.
    Rousseau L; Rousseau R
    Percept Psychophys; 1996 Apr; 58(3):434-48. PubMed ID: 8935904
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Perception and production of brief durations: beat-based versus interval-based timing.
    Pashler H
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2001 Apr; 27(2):485-93. PubMed ID: 11318062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Detection of linear temporal drift in sound sequences: empirical data and modelling principles.
    Madison G
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2004 Sep; 117(1):95-118. PubMed ID: 15288231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Prospective and retrospective time estimates as a function of clock duration.
    Predebon J
    Percept Mot Skills; 1995 Jun; 80(3 Pt 1):941-2. PubMed ID: 7567415
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Anchoring in time estimation.
    Thomas KE; Handley SJ
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2008 Jan; 127(1):24-9. PubMed ID: 17276378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Visual estimation of filled or unfilled short time intervals.
    Shekerdjiiski S
    Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg; 1979; 5(2):62-6. PubMed ID: 517143
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Eye movements are not a prerequisite for learning movement sequence timing through observation.
    Hayes SJ; Timmis MA; Bennett SJ
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2009 Jul; 131(3):202-8. PubMed ID: 19500770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Tracking time with song and count: different Weber functions for musicians and nonmusicians.
    Grondin S; Killeen PR
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2009 Oct; 71(7):1649-54. PubMed ID: 19801624
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Test sequence priming in recognition memory.
    Johns EE; Mewhort DJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2009 Sep; 35(5):1162-74. PubMed ID: 19686012
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Using the attention cascade model to probe cognitive aging.
    Shih SI
    Psychol Aging; 2009 Sep; 24(3):550-62. PubMed ID: 19739911
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. On the replication of Kristofferson's (1980) quantal timing for duration discrimination: some learning but no quanta and not much of a Weber constant.
    Matthews WJ; Grondin S
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2012 Jul; 74(5):1056-72. PubMed ID: 22391892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Priming of simple and complex scene layout: rapid function from the intermediate level.
    Sanocki T; Sulman N
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2009 Jun; 35(3):735-49. PubMed ID: 19485688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. The time of our lives: life span development of timing and event tracking.
    McAuley JD; Jones MR; Holub S; Johnston HM; Miller NS
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2006 Aug; 135(3):348-67. PubMed ID: 16846269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Conflicts during response selection affect response programming: reactions toward the source of stimulation.
    Buetti S; Kerzel D
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2009 Jun; 35(3):816-34. PubMed ID: 19485694
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. How emotions colour our perception of time.
    Droit-Volet S; Meck WH
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2007 Dec; 11(12):504-13. PubMed ID: 18023604
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Extremely selective attention: eye-tracking studies of the dynamic allocation of attention to stimulus features in categorization.
    Blair MR; Watson MR; Walshe RC; Maj F
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2009 Sep; 35(5):1196-206. PubMed ID: 19686015
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Estimation of duration of an array of time interval and of its separate elements.
    Shekerdjiiski S; Mitrani L; Gurevitch A
    Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg; 1977; 3(2):76-81. PubMed ID: 596177
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Modeling effects of rhythmic context on perceived duration: a comparison of interval and entrainment approaches to short-interval timing.
    McAuley JD; Jones MR
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2003 Dec; 29(6):1102-25. PubMed ID: 14640833
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Can attentional theory explain the inverse base rate effect? Comment on Kruschke (2001).
    Winman A; Wennerholm P; Juslin P
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2003 Nov; 29(6):1390-5; discussion 1396-1400. PubMed ID: 14622069
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Can intertrial effects of features and dimensions be explained by a single theory?
    Becker SI
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2008 Dec; 34(6):1417-40. PubMed ID: 19045983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.