BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

249 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19034502)

  • 1. Individual differences in conflict-monitoring: testing means and covariance hypothesis about the Simon and the Eriksen Flanker task.
    Keye D; Wilhelm O; Oberauer K; van Ravenzwaaij D
    Psychol Res; 2009 Nov; 73(6):762-76. PubMed ID: 19034502
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Opposing influences on conflict-driven adaptation in the Eriksen flanker task.
    Bugg JM
    Mem Cognit; 2008 Oct; 36(7):1217-27. PubMed ID: 18927038
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The influence of reward in the Simon task: Differences and similarities to the Stroop and Eriksen flanker tasks.
    Mittelstädt V; Ulrich R; König J; Hofbauer K; Mackenzie IG
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2023 Apr; 85(3):949-959. PubMed ID: 36316615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Sequential dependencies in the Eriksen flanker task: a direct comparison of two competing accounts.
    Davelaar EJ; Stevens J
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2009 Feb; 16(1):121-6. PubMed ID: 19145021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Adjustments to recent and frequent conflict reflect two distinguishable mechanisms.
    Purmann S; Badde S; Wendt M
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2009 Apr; 16(2):350-5. PubMed ID: 19293106
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Are spatial and dimensional attention separate? evidence from Posner, Stroop, and Eriksen tasks.
    Chajut E; Schupak A; Algom D
    Mem Cognit; 2009 Sep; 37(6):924-34. PubMed ID: 19679870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Slow and steady? Strategic adjustments in response caution are moderately reliable and correlate across tasks.
    Hedge C; Vivian-Griffiths S; Powell G; Bompas A; Sumner P
    Conscious Cogn; 2019 Oct; 75():102797. PubMed ID: 31421398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. An Investigation of the Reliability and Self-Regulatory Correlates of Conflict Adaptation.
    Feldman JL; Freitas AL
    Exp Psychol; 2016 Jul; 63(4):237-247. PubMed ID: 27750519
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Effects of conflict trial proportion: A comparison of the Eriksen and Simon tasks.
    Bausenhart KM; Ulrich R; Miller J
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2021 Feb; 83(2):810-836. PubMed ID: 33269440
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Interacting congruency effects in the hybrid Stroop-Simon task prevent conclusions regarding the domain specificity or generality of the congruency sequence effect.
    Weissman DH
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2020 May; 46(5):945-967. PubMed ID: 31580121
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Conflict control of emotional and non-emotional conflicts in preadolescent children.
    Liu T; Liu X; Li D; Shangguan F; Lu L; Shi J
    Biol Psychol; 2019 Sep; 146():107708. PubMed ID: 31153934
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Conflict-frequency affects flanker interference: role of stimulus-ensemble-specific practiceand flanker-response contingencies.
    Wendt M; Luna-Rodriguez A
    Exp Psychol; 2009; 56(3):206-17. PubMed ID: 19289363
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A spatial version of the Stroop task for examining proactive and reactive control independently from non-conflict processes.
    Spinelli G; Lupker SJ
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2024 May; 86(4):1259-1286. PubMed ID: 38691237
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Conflict resolution in the Eriksen flanker task: Similarities and differences to the Simon task.
    Hübner R; Töbel L
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0214203. PubMed ID: 30921368
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Dissociation of S-R compatibility and Simon effects with mixed tasks and mappings.
    Proctor RW; Yamaguchi M; Dutt V; Gonzalez C
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2013 Apr; 39(2):593-609. PubMed ID: 22963231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Generality and specificity in cognitive control: conflict adaptation within and across selective-attention tasks but not across selective-attention and Simon tasks.
    Freitas AL; Clark SL
    Psychol Res; 2015 Jan; 79(1):143-62. PubMed ID: 24487727
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Cognitive control controls the effect of irrelevant stimulus-response learning.
    Shi K; Wang L
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2024 Apr; 86(3):866-882. PubMed ID: 38413504
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Accounting for sequential trial effects in the flanker task: conflict adaptation or associative priming?
    Nieuwenhuis S; Stins JF; Posthuma D; Polderman TJ; Boomsma DI; de Geus EJ
    Mem Cognit; 2006 Sep; 34(6):1260-72. PubMed ID: 17225507
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The role of task-relevant and task-irrelevant information in congruency sequence effects: Applying the diffusion model for conflict tasks.
    Koob V; Mackenzie I; Ulrich R; Leuthold H; Janczyk M
    Cogn Psychol; 2023 Feb; 140():101528. PubMed ID: 36584549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Conflict Adaptation and Cue Competition during Learning in an Eriksen Flanker Task.
    Ghinescu R; Schachtman TR; Ramsey AK; Gratton G; Fabiani M
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(12):e0167119. PubMed ID: 27941977
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.