These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

135 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19120417)

  • 21. Rigid thinking about deformables: do children sometimes overgeneralize the shape bias?
    Samuelson LK; Horst JS; Schutte AR; Dobbertin BN
    J Child Lang; 2008 Aug; 35(3):559-89. PubMed ID: 18588715
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Sample size, number of categories and sampling assumptions: Exploring some differences between categorization and generalization.
    Hendrickson AT; Perfors A; Navarro DJ; Ransom K
    Cogn Psychol; 2019 Jun; 111():80-102. PubMed ID: 30947074
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Categorical relations in shape perception.
    Hummel JE; Stankiewicz BJ
    Spat Vis; 1996; 10(3):201-36. PubMed ID: 9061832
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The role of actions in making inferences about the shape and material of solid objects among Japanese 2 year-old children.
    Kobayashi H
    Cognition; 1997 Jun; 63(3):251-69. PubMed ID: 9265871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Infants' attention to object structure in early categorization.
    Rakison DH; Butterworth GE
    Dev Psychol; 1998 Nov; 34(6):1310-25. PubMed ID: 9823514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The development of induction based on noun and feature labels.
    Sweller N; Hayes BK
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2014 Aug; 21(4):1048-55. PubMed ID: 24395097
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. [Individual and family factors influencing vocabulary development in toddlers].
    Hao B; Liang WL; Wang S; Ji CY; Zhang ZX; Zuo QH; Tardif T; Fletcher P
    Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi; 2004 Dec; 42(12):908-12. PubMed ID: 15733359
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Infant object categorization transcends diverse object-context relations.
    Bornstein MH; Arterberry ME; Mash C
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Feb; 33(1):7-15. PubMed ID: 20031232
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. By land or by sea: the role of perceptual similarity in infants' categorization of animals.
    Oakes LM; Coppage DJ; Dingel A
    Dev Psychol; 1997 May; 33(3):396-407. PubMed ID: 9149919
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Developmental changes in visual object recognition between 18 and 24 months of age.
    Pereira AF; Smith LB
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):67-80. PubMed ID: 19120414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Developmental changes in children's understanding of the similarity between photographs and their referents.
    Uttal DH; Gentner D; Liu LL; Lewis AR
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jan; 11(1):156-70. PubMed ID: 18171376
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Sensitivity of 24-month-olds to the prior inaccuracy of the source: possible mechanisms.
    Koenig MA; Woodward AL
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Jul; 46(4):815-26. PubMed ID: 20604604
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Categorization, categorical perception, and asymmetry in infants' representation of face race.
    Anzures G; Quinn PC; Pascalis O; Slater AM; Lee K
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jul; 13(4):553-64. PubMed ID: 20590720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Is a Pink Cow Still a Cow? Individual Differences in Toddlers' Vocabulary Knowledge and Lexical Representations.
    Perry LK; Saffran JR
    Cogn Sci; 2017 May; 41(4):1090-1105. PubMed ID: 27059812
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Shared understanding and idiosyncratic expression in early vocabularies.
    Mayor J; Plunkett K
    Dev Sci; 2014 May; 17(3):412-23. PubMed ID: 24410758
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Core knowledge and its limits: the domain of food.
    Shutts K; Condry KF; Santos LR; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2009 Jul; 112(1):120-40. PubMed ID: 19409538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Why is a pomegranate an apple? The role of shape, taxonomic relatedness, and prior lexical knowledge in children's overextensions of apple and dog.
    Gelman SA; Croft W; Fu P; Clausner T; Gottfried G
    J Child Lang; 1998 Jun; 25(2):267-91. PubMed ID: 9770908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Toddlers' joint engagement experience facilitates preschoolers' acquisition of theory of mind.
    Nelson PB; Adamson LB; Bakeman R
    Dev Sci; 2008 Nov; 11(6):847-52. PubMed ID: 19046153
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The Role of Generic Language in the Early Development of Social Categorization.
    Rhodes M; Leslie SJ; Bianchi L; Chalik L
    Child Dev; 2018 Jan; 89(1):148-155. PubMed ID: 28129455
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.