BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

355 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19236143)

  • 1. Informative breath: olfactory cues sought during social foraging among Old World monkeys (Mandrillus sphinx, M. Leucophaeus, and Papio anubis).
    Laidre ME
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Feb; 123(1):34-44. PubMed ID: 19236143
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Social cues influence foraging in dwarf hamsters (Phodopus campbelli).
    Lupfer-Johnson G; Hanson KL; Edwards LE; Elder RL; Evans SL
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 May; 123(2):226-9. PubMed ID: 19450030
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Tonkean macaques ( Macaca tonkeana) find food sources from cues conveyed by group-mates.
    Drapier M; Chauvin C; Thierry B
    Anim Cogn; 2002 Sep; 5(3):159-65. PubMed ID: 12357288
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Use of spatial, visual, and olfactory information during foraging in wild nocturnal and diurnal anthropoids: A field experiment comparing Aotus, Callicebus, and Saguinus.
    Bicca-Marques JC; Garber PA
    Am J Primatol; 2004 Mar; 62(3):171-87. PubMed ID: 15027091
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Do captive mandrills invent new gestures?
    Laidre ME
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Apr; 11(2):179-87. PubMed ID: 18008095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Which senses play a role in nonhuman primate food selection? A comparison between squirrel monkeys and spider monkeys.
    Laska M; Freist P; Krause S
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Mar; 69(3):282-94. PubMed ID: 17146790
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Nasopalatine ducts and flehmen behavior in the mandrill: reevaluating olfactory communication in Old World primates.
    Charpentier MJ; Mboumba S; Ditsoga C; Drea CM
    Am J Primatol; 2013 Jul; 75(7):703-14. PubMed ID: 23526642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Monitoring of group mates in relation to their behavior in mandrills.
    Schino G; Scerbo M
    Am J Primatol; 2020 Jun; 82(6):e23129. PubMed ID: 32239768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Circannual changes in the secondary sexual adornments of semifree-ranging male and female mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx).
    Setchell JM; Dixson AF
    Am J Primatol; 2001 Mar; 53(3):109-21. PubMed ID: 11253846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Changes in social interactions during adolescence in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx).
    Charpentier M; Peignot P; Hossaert-Mckey M; Wickings EJ
    Am J Primatol; 2004 Jun; 63(2):63-73. PubMed ID: 15195328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Associations between the drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus) and sympatric monkeys in Korup National Park, Cameroon.
    Astaras C; Krause S; Mattner L; Rehse C; Waltert M
    Am J Primatol; 2011 Feb; 73(2):127-34. PubMed ID: 20853399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Relationship between reluctance to eat novel foods and open-field behavior in sheep.
    Villalba JJ; Manteca X; Provenza FD
    Physiol Behav; 2009 Feb; 96(2):276-81. PubMed ID: 18957301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Contrast of hand preferences between communicative gestures and non-communicative actions in baboons: implications for the origins of hemispheric specialization for language.
    Meguerditchian A; Vauclair J
    Brain Lang; 2009 Mar; 108(3):167-74. PubMed ID: 19091390
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. From frugivore to folivore: Altitudinal variations in the diet and feeding ecology of the Bioko Island drill (Mandrillus leucophaeus poensis).
    Owens JR; Honarvar S; Nessel M; Hearn GW
    Am J Primatol; 2015 Dec; 77(12):1263-75. PubMed ID: 26375479
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Susceptibility of artificially reared rat pups to social influences on food choice.
    Galef BG; Smith MA
    Dev Psychobiol; 1994 Mar; 27(2):85-92. PubMed ID: 8187970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Life history in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx): physical development, dominance rank, and group association.
    Setchell JM; Wickings EJ; Knapp LA
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2006 Dec; 131(4):498-510. PubMed ID: 16958078
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Social interaction with non-averse group-mates modifies a learned food aversion in single- and mixed-species groups of tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and S. labiatus).
    Prescott MJ; Buchanan-Smith HM; Smith AC
    Am J Primatol; 2005 Apr; 65(4):313-26. PubMed ID: 15834888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) do not utilize social information in three simultaneous social foraging tasks.
    Voelkl B; Huber L
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):149-58. PubMed ID: 16969636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Comparative performance of orangutans (Pongo spp.), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), and drills (Mandrillus leucophaeus), in an ephemeral foraging task.
    Prétôt L; Mickelberg J; Carrigan J; Stoinski T; Bshary R; Brosnan SF
    Am J Primatol; 2021 Jan; 83(1):e23212. PubMed ID: 33135209
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Response toward novel stimuli in a group of tufted capuchins (Cebus libidinosus) in Brasília National Park, Brazil.
    Sabbatini G; Stammati M; Tavares MC; Visalberghi E
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Apr; 69(4):457-70. PubMed ID: 17216622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 18.