These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

228 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19236398)

  • 21. Word meaning and the control of eye fixation: semantic competitor effects and the visual world paradigm.
    Huettig F; Altmann GT
    Cognition; 2005 May; 96(1):B23-32. PubMed ID: 15833303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Producing and processing self-propelled motion in infancy.
    Cicchino JB; Rakison DH
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Sep; 44(5):1232-41. PubMed ID: 18793057
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Young children associate novel words with complex objects rather than salient parts.
    Hollich G; Golinkoff RM; Hirsh-Pasek K
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1051-61. PubMed ID: 17723035
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Infants' use of synchronized visual information to separate streams of speech.
    Hollich G; Newman RS; Jusczyk PW
    Child Dev; 2005; 76(3):598-613. PubMed ID: 15892781
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Infants' intermodal perception of canine (Canis familairis) facial expressions and vocalizations.
    Flom R; Whipple H; Hyde D
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Jul; 45(4):1143-51. PubMed ID: 19586184
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Location, location, location: development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy.
    Kirkham NZ; Slemmer JA; Richardson DC; Johnson SP
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(5):1559-71. PubMed ID: 17883448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Rapid word learning by fifteen-month-olds under tightly controlled conditions.
    Schafer G; Plunkett K
    Child Dev; 1998 Apr; 69(2):309-20. PubMed ID: 9586207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Head and eye movements affect object processing in 4-month-old infants more than an artificial orientation cue.
    Wahl S; Michel C; Pauen S; Hoehl S
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2013 Jun; 31(Pt 2):212-30. PubMed ID: 23659892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Perceiving "outside the box" occurs early in development: evidence for boundary extension in three- to seven-month-old infants.
    Quinn PC; Intraub H
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(1):324-34. PubMed ID: 17328708
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Effects of the similarity and dissimilarity between familiarization and test objects on recognition memory in infants following unimodal and bimodal familiarization.
    Rolfe SA; Day RH
    Child Dev; 1981; 52(4):1308-12. PubMed ID: 7318525
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. The time-course of attentional bias in anxious children and adolescents.
    Gamble AL; Rapee RM
    J Anxiety Disord; 2009 Oct; 23(7):841-7. PubMed ID: 19447004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Qualitative differences in the exploration of upright and upside-down faces in four-month-old infants: an eye-movement study.
    Gallay M; Baudouin JY; Durand K; Lemoine C; Lécuyer R
    Child Dev; 2006; 77(4):984-96. PubMed ID: 16942501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. [Categorization in infancy: differentiation of global object classes].
    Pauen S
    Z Exp Psychol; 1996; 43(4):600-24. PubMed ID: 9206586
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Do squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) predict that looking leads to touching?
    Anderson JR; Kuroshima H; Kuwahata H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jul; 7(3):185-92. PubMed ID: 15022054
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiaris) show situation-dependent recognition of human's attention.
    Gácsi M; Miklósi A; Varga O; Topál J; Csányi V
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jul; 7(3):144-53. PubMed ID: 14669075
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants.
    Quinn PC; Eimas PD; Rosenkrantz SL
    Perception; 1993; 22(4):463-75. PubMed ID: 8378134
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Perceptual cues that permit categorical differentiation of animal species by infants.
    Quinn PC; Eimas PD
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1996 Oct; 63(1):189-211. PubMed ID: 8812045
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Stimulus exposure and gaze bias: a further test of the gaze cascade model.
    Glaholt MG; Reingold EM
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2009 Apr; 71(3):445-50. PubMed ID: 19304635
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The attribution of attention: 9-month-olds' interpretation of gaze as goal-directed action.
    Johnson SC; Ok SJ; Luo Y
    Dev Sci; 2007 Sep; 10(5):530-7. PubMed ID: 17683339
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Dogs do look at images: eye tracking in canine cognition research.
    Somppi S; Törnqvist H; Hänninen L; Krause C; Vainio O
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):163-74. PubMed ID: 21861109
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.