These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
256 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19327760)
1. Harnessing the wandering mind: the role of perceptual load. Forster S; Lavie N Cognition; 2009 Jun; 111(3):345-55. PubMed ID: 19327760 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Distracted by your mind? Individual differences in distractibility predict mind wandering. Forster S; Lavie N J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2014 Jan; 40(1):251-260. PubMed ID: 23957365 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. The influence of time on task on mind wandering and visual working memory. Krimsky M; Forster DE; Llabre MM; Jha AP Cognition; 2017 Dec; 169():84-90. PubMed ID: 28865286 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Perceptual load interacts with stimulus processing across sensory modalities. Klemen J; Büchel C; Rose M Eur J Neurosci; 2009 Jun; 29(12):2426-34. PubMed ID: 19490081 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. The role of task difficulty in theoretical accounts of mind wandering. Seli P; Konishi M; Risko EF; Smilek D Conscious Cogn; 2018 Oct; 65():255-262. PubMed ID: 30219746 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The role of working memory in auditory selective attention. Dalton P; Santangelo V; Spence C Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Nov; 62(11):2126-32. PubMed ID: 19557667 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Task-unrelated thought depends on the phonological short-term memory system more than the visual short-term memory system. Sekiguchi T Acta Psychol (Amst); 2018 Oct; 190():228-238. PubMed ID: 30149237 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. The role of perceptual load in neglect: rejection of ipsilesional distractors is facilitated with higher central load. Lavie N; Robertson IH J Cogn Neurosci; 2001 Oct; 13(7):867-76. PubMed ID: 11595091 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Components of working memory and visual selective attention. Burnham BR; Sabia M; Langan C J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2014 Feb; 40(1):391-403. PubMed ID: 23875574 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Working memory capacity does not always support future-oriented mind-wandering. McVay JC; Unsworth N; McMillan BD; Kane MJ Can J Exp Psychol; 2013 Mar; 67(1):41-50. PubMed ID: 23458550 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Understanding the allocation of attention when faced with varying perceptual load in partial report: a computational approach. Kyllingsbæk S; Sy JL; Giesbrecht B Neuropsychologia; 2011 May; 49(6):1487-97. PubMed ID: 21145901 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Interference from familiar natural distractors is not eliminated by high perceptual load. He C; Chen A Psychol Res; 2010 May; 74(3):268-76. PubMed ID: 19652997 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: the role of load. Forster S; Lavie N J Exp Psychol Appl; 2008 Mar; 14(1):73-83. PubMed ID: 18377168 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought. Seli P; Cheyne JA; Xu M; Purdon C; Smilek D J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2015 Sep; 41(5):1417-1425. PubMed ID: 25730306 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Auditory working memory load impairs visual ventral stream processing: toward a unified model of attentional load. Klemen J; Büchel C; Bühler M; Menz MM; Rose M J Cogn Neurosci; 2010 Mar; 22(3):437-46. PubMed ID: 19302005 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]