These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

151 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 19542092)

  • 1. Overshadowing and CS duration: counteraction and a reexamination of the role of within-compound associations in cue competition.
    Sissons HT; Urcelay GP; Miller RR
    Learn Behav; 2009 Aug; 37(3):254-68. PubMed ID: 19542092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Overshadowing of subsequent events and recovery thereafter.
    Burger DC; Mallemat H; Miller RR
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 2000 May; 53(2):149-71. PubMed ID: 10881606
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Posttraining shifts in the overshadowing stimulus-unconditioned stimulus interval alleviates the overshadowing deficit.
    Blaisdell AP; Denniston JC; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1999 Jan; 25(1):18-27. PubMed ID: 9987855
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Potentiation and overshadowing in Pavlovian fear conditioning.
    Urcelay GP; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Jul; 35(3):340-56. PubMed ID: 19594280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Effects of an extinguished CS on competition with another CS.
    Bills CH; Dopheide M; Pineño O; Schachtman TR
    Behav Processes; 2006 Mar; 72(1):14-22. PubMed ID: 16457968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Overshadowing and stimulus duration.
    Jennings DJ; Bonardi C; Kirkpatrick K
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Oct; 33(4):464-75. PubMed ID: 17924793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Stimulus competition between a discrete cue and a training context: Cue competition does not result from the division of a limited resource.
    Urushihara K; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Apr; 35(2):197-211. PubMed ID: 19364229
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Effects of extinction of a nontarget CS on performance to a target CS.
    Richardson RA; Michener PN; Schachtman TR
    Behav Processes; 2018 Sep; 154():13-20. PubMed ID: 29499345
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Responding to a conditioned stimulus depends on the current associative status of other cues present during training of that specific stimulus.
    Miller RR; Barnet RC; Grahame NJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1992 Jul; 18(3):251-64. PubMed ID: 1619393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Trial spacing is a determinant of cue interaction.
    Stout SC; Chang R; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2003 Jan; 29(1):23-38. PubMed ID: 12561131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Contrasting reduced overshadowing and blocking.
    Wheeler DS; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Jul; 33(3):349-59. PubMed ID: 17620032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Interaction between preexposure and overshadowing: further analysis of the extended comparator hypothesis.
    Savastano HI; Arcediano F; Stout SC; Miller RR
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 2003 Nov; 56(4):371-95. PubMed ID: 14578081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Overshadowing by fixed- and variable-duration stimuli.
    Bonardi C; Mondragón E; Brilot B; Jennings DJ
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2015; 68(3):523-42. PubMed ID: 25203812
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Conditioned inhibition produced by extinction-mediated recovery from the relative stimulus validity effect: a test of acquisition and performance models of empirical retrospective revaluation.
    Blaisdell AP; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2001 Jan; 27(1):48-58. PubMed ID: 11199514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. An inhibitory within-compound association attenuates overshadowing.
    Amundson JC; Witnauer JE; Pineño O; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2008 Jan; 34(1):133-43. PubMed ID: 18248120
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Degraded contingency revisited: posttraining extinction of a cover stimulus attenuates a target cue's behavioral control.
    Witnauer JE; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Oct; 33(4):440-50. PubMed ID: 17924791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Counterconditioning of an overshadowed cue attenuates overshadowing.
    Blaisdell AP; Denniston JC; Savastano HI; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2000 Jan; 26(1):74-86. PubMed ID: 10650545
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Counteraction between overshadowing and degraded contingency treatments: support for the extended comparator hypothesis.
    Urcelay GP; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2006 Jan; 32(1):21-32. PubMed ID: 16435962
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Some determinants of second-order conditioning.
    Witnauer JE; Miller RR
    Learn Behav; 2011 Mar; 39(1):12-26. PubMed ID: 21264564
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Prevention of the degraded-contingency effect by signalling training trials.
    Gunther LM; Miller RR
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 2000 May; 53(2):97-119. PubMed ID: 10881603
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.